Introduction

The use of assisted reproductive technologies (ART) has been increasing dramatically over the last 40 years, due to advancements in science and technology, fertility decreases in young women, delayed childbearing, shifts in societal attitudes, changes in legislation, among others [1, 2]. It is estimated that since the birth of the first “test-tube” baby in 1978, over 8 million children have been born through ART [3]. According to the European Society of Human Reproduction and Embryology, 157,500 children were born via ART in 2015 [4]. These procedures can be described as a wide range of medical techniques that are used to, primarily, help infertile couples conceive. A variety of ART treatments are available. In some of these procedures, children have both a gestational and a genetic connection to their mother, father and the mother that undergoes the pregnancy. This is the case of homologous in vitro fertilization (IVF; fertilization of an egg and sperm in a Petri dish) and homologous intracytoplasmic sperm injection (ICSI; the injection of a single sperm directly into a mature egg). For other procedures—included in the reproduction donation—children can be genetically related only to the mother, if a donor sperm is used (donor insemination; DI); only to the father via egg donation (ED); or to neither parent when both the donor egg and sperm are used) (embryo donation) [5]. Parents may also choose surrogacy, in which the pregnancy is carried by another woman. In this case, depending on the specific arrangement, children can either be genetically related to only one parent, both parents, or neither [5]. The various possibilities allowed by the available techniques of reproductive donation involve different genetic relatedness to mother and/or father, which may imply dissimilar effects on children’s psychological adjustment. The majority of the studies reported finding no significant differences in children’s psychological adjustment between different ART children; nevertheless, the different particularities and family processes associated with the absence of genetic relatedness to mother and/or father and its impact on children’s psychological adjustment remain unexplored.

Moreover, reproduction donation is becoming an increasingly popular method for conceiving children not only by infertile couples, but also by same-gender couples or single parents [6]. For this reason, by the end of the twenty-first century, new family forms began to appear. The term “new families”, proposed by Golombok [7], distinguishes these from other “non-traditional families”, such as those formed through cohabitation or single parenthood, about which there is already a considerable amount of literature [7, 8]. A heterosexual, cisgender couple raising children that are genetically related to them is considered the “traditional family” paradigm. In that sense, new families may present different structures in terms of number, gender, and parents’ sexual orientation, as well as in their genetic and gestational relatedness with the children [1]. Solo mothers, also known as single mothers by choice, are a relatively new type of “non-traditional family” that is formed by single women who actively decide to create a family without having a partner [9]. For example, in 2017 about one-in-five children (21%) in the USA were living with a solo mother [10]. Several studies show that single mothers by choice are usually well-educated women in professional occupations who become mothers in their late 30 s or early 40 s. Many choose to become single mothers not because they do not want a traditional family setting, but due to their increasing age and its associated fertility decline [8].

Despite that lesbian-parented families may differ from the previous family configuration because of the mothers’ sexual orientation, they share the similarity of children being raised by women without the presence of a father. The increase in births among lesbian women in the USA has been so enormous and many designated it as a “lesbian baby boom” [11]. According to a 2001 US survey, 74% of lesbian couples are treated in ART facilities [12]. Operationalizing the concept of parenthood for lesbian-parented (and gay-parented) families is complex, since there are many possibilities for the parental status. For instance, for a lesbian couple with a child conceived through sperm donation, the non-biological mother is recognized as a mother due to the emotional connection, but the lack of biological connection could implicate a lack of legal status regarding the child, depending on the country’s legal context. This ambiguity interferes with the estimates of the prevalence of lesbian-parented families and to access a total description of those families [13]. Nonetheless, it is estimated that, in 2014, in the USA, 157,700 children were being raised by lesbian-parented families, in addition to 150,000 children being raised by a lesbian single parent [14].

In gay-parented families, a more recent “new family” configuration, children are raised by gay couples or single gay parents, although there are fewer studies about gay-parented families because most children in same-gender households are being raised by lesbian mothers. Most of the research on gay-parented families has focused on those formed through surrogacy, in which the parents can select an “open-identity donor” (with whom they might have contact in the future) or an “anonymous donor” (with whom they want little or no contact) [15]. In 2014, in the USA, around 32,300 children were being raised by gay couples, in addition to 60,000 children being raised by a gay single parent [14].

Lastly, trans-parented families are the least known family configurations. Nevertheless, survey data show that between 25 and 49% of trans individuals are parents, even if children may be born before or after the parent’s transition. Trans parents are, generally, older than trans individuals without children and the prevalence of parenthood tends to be higher among trans women than among trans men [16].

In recent decades, there has been a considerable increase in non-traditional families. Therefore, an increasing body of research on the medical outcomes of children born through ART has emerged out of concerns for the well-being of children. Consequently, several studies have started to focus on the psychological adjustment of children born through ART [17]. Psychological adjustment is described as an individual's general sentiments of well-being and pleasure as a result of reduced stress in their daily lives [18]. Further, Ward and Kennedy [19] described psychological adjustment in terms of emotional and affective results, including mental health concerns (e.g., depressive symptoms, temper disturbances, etc.) [19]. Because ART households differ from conventional families, extra concerns regarding children’s psychological adjustment arise, due to the distinct pattern of gestational and genetic connections between people inside and outside the nuclear family. In that sense, planned families through ART are often victims of social criticism and stigma. For example, due to the traditional belief that “a child needs a father”, there is a tendency to suppose that children from solo mothers are not as psychologically adjusted as those in traditional households [20]. Similar preconceived notions affect DI families, especially in cases where the donor’s identity is unknown to the child as they grow up. The secrecy of the child’s biological origins is thought to result in negative psychological consequences for children [21].

Further, concerns regarding gay-parented families are also highly prevalent. This is due to the fact that fathers are rarely the primary caregivers and are often seen as less nurturing and capable of parenting than women [22]. Particularly gay-parented families that use surrogacy are often stigmatized, as many assume that the child may view the surrogate as a mother and suffer from the absence of this relationship, or feel abandoned by her [15]. In contrast to the popular belief, a meta-analysis of the impact of gay fathers on children's psychological adjustment discovered that when compared to children of heterosexual parents’ children of gay fathers might even do better in some psychological sectors, notably displaying fewer internalizing and externalizing problems [23].

Finally, Golombok [24] has recently developed a theoretical review about the psychological adjustment of children born through ART and concluded that gender, number, genetic relatedness, and sexual orientation of parents do not impact children’s psychological adjustment [24]. Instead, similar to those in traditional households, the psychological adjustment of children raised in ART families appear to rely upon the parents’ well-being, the nature and quality of relationships inside the family, the assistance of their wider community and the social occasions they are raised in [24]. Although the authors have included several studies on the field in the last 40 years, it was not systematically conducted. A systematic review was indeed performed by the mentioned author [25]; however, it was a review focused on adolescents and only with studies published in English. Therefore, it is important to systematically summarize the immensity of the data about the psychological adjustment of ART children under 11 years old and to assess the quality of studies. Furthermore, many of the studies in the field have only performed intra-samples comparisons (ART children versus natural conceived children and adoptive children) which could add an important bias to the study’s conclusions. A confirmation that all these studied children are in fact well adjusted, when compared with normative data for the main indexes of mental health is lacking. It is important to perform a mental health screening of the ART children across all studies (when possible) enabling a homogenization of the information regardless of the heterogeneity of the methodologies, samples, sampling methods, countries, and informants.

In sum, to the best of our knowledge, no systematic review has examined the psychological impact that being born through ART has on children, and if ART children’s psychological adjustment could be considered similar to that of NC children.

The general objective of the present study is to systematically assess the psychological adjustment of children aged between 3 and 11 years born through ART. The first specific objective is to compare ART children’s scores on standardized indexes of mental health with normative data to examine ART children’s overall psychological adjustment. The second specific objective is to examine possible differences in children’s psychological adjustment across different ART, and especially those involving reproductive donation (DI, embryo donation, egg donation, sperm donation and surrogacy) where the child is not genetically related to at least one parent. Lastly, the third specific objective is to examine possible differences in children’s psychological adjustment across different family configurations, namely, between children from single mothers/fathers’ families versus children from two-parent families; children from lesbian-parented families and/or gay-parented families versus children from heterosexual-parented families; and children from transgender-parented families versus children from cisgender-parented families. Summarizing the mentioned literature will help to identify what is known about children’s psychological health in ART families, as well as to help identify gaps in the literature for future research in this field.

Methods

Search strategy

A systematic review of children’s psychological adjustment in ART families was developed. The systematic search followed PRISMA guidelines [26]. A literature search was conducted independently by two researchers (V. L. and S. N.) using the APA PsycInfo, APA PsycArticles, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, Academic Search Complete, Pubmed® (National Library of Medicine), Scopus® (Elsevier), Web of Science version 5.35 (Clarivate Analytics), Scielo (Scientific Electronic Library Online) and RCAAP (Repositórios Cientificos de Acesso Aberto de Portugal) (see Table 1). Search terms are described in Table 1. Search terms also incorporated some MeSH terms and comprised all possible key words related to ART and children’s psychological adjustment. Additional studies were identified through the lists of references of the included studies and through references from field expert’s curriculum.

Table 1 Search and study selection for a systematic review on children’s psychological adjustment in ART families

Study selection

Gamete/embryo donation and surrogacy are relatively recent practices—as mentioned by Ilioi and Golombok [7]—and to our knowledge, no previous review has examined the psychological outcomes of children from ART; therefore, we decided not to impose any limits to time of publication. Thus, the literature search was conducted from inception through September 2021. Inclusion and exclusion criteria are described in Table 1. Articles that used the following terms for psychological adjustment were also included: child development, emotional and/or behavioural adjustment, socioemotional development, child adjustment, psychological development, and well-being. ART was defined as IVF, ICSI, donor insemination, gamete donation, egg/oocyte donation, sperm donation, embryo donation and surrogacy. This review was not limited to studies published in English, but has also searched for studies published in four other languages (Portuguese, Italian, French and Spanish) and was focused on children aged between 3 and 11 years not only to expand upon [25] systematic review that focused on the psychological adjustment of adolescents born through ART (11–18 years old), but also to focus throughout all childhood, which includes the early childhood (3–6 years) and middle childhood (6–11) as defined by [27].

Screening, quality assessment, data extraction and data synthesis

As shown in Fig. 1, a total of 337 results were obtained. Two independent authors (V.L. and S.N.) performed an initial inspection of the titles and abstracts of each study. Duplicates (n = 44) were removed and additional studies from other sources (n = 14) were included. The 279 studies were then assessed in accordance with the inclusion and exclusion criteria. From an initial screening, 229 studies were excluded based on the title and/or the abstract. This process was repeated by the two mentioned authors to confirm the first inspection. The remaining 50 studies were thorough analysed independently by three authors (F.A.C., V.L. and S.N.) and after applying the exclusion criteria five studies were excluded. Lastly, the authors independently revised the remaining articles to confirm their eligibility and performed a formal quality assessment (see Appendix A available in the OSF, at https://osf.io/pjnbk/?view_only=92979f986dfc419297eed6a8847cda1e). The quality assessment final table was developed by the authors based on available scientific criteria to assess empirical studies' quality and was also based on some criteria of the Critical Appraisal Skills Programme standards [28]. Since there are no CASP criteria specifically for quality assessment of quantitative studies, we used some criteria of qualitative studies and adapted to both qualitative and quantitative studies to evaluate all included articles. Divergences were discussed and resolved by consensus among three authors (F.A.C., V.L. and S.N.). The present systematic review included 45 studies.

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram for the systematic review of children’s psychological adjustment in ART families

The publication year, descriptive data details (country, ART type, family configuration, children’s age, measures, psychological adjustment definitions), study design and main outcomes were extracted and summarized. All measures extracted for each study were organized by informant (parents, teachers and/or children). Regarding the measures of psychological adjustment, the vast majority of the studies used the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) and ASEBA forms (CBCL and TRF) and the few other measures extracted were the Rutter scales, the Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence, the Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory (ECBI), the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale and the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS). The mean scores (and standard deviation) of the SDQ and ASEBA forms (CBCL and TRF) were also extracted (see Appendix B available in the OSF, at https://osf.io/pjnbk/?view_only=92979f986dfc419297eed6a8847cda1e). Specifically, the scores of the Internalizing (Emotional Problems and Peer scales), Externalizing conduct and Hyperactivity/Inattention scales) and total difficulties scales of the SDQ. For the ASEBA forms, the scales were also the Internalizing, Externalizing and Total Behavioural Problems. Data extraction was performed independently by two independent authors (V.L. and S.N.) and later two other authors (F.A.C. and P.A.C) revised and confirmed the extracted information.

Concerning data synthesis, the SDQ and ASEBA mean scores of the included studies were organized to perform a comparative analysis with normative data for all studies, including those that only used comparison samples (e.g. naturally conceived children). Finally, using the program MAXQDA (2020), we organized the data of the main outcomes into the following sections and subsections: (a) ART children and different ART; and (b) ART children and family configurations, which included (b.1) the number of parents and ART children’ psychological adjustment; (b.2) lesbian-, gay- and heterosexual-parented families and psychological adjustment; and (b.3) trans parents and ART children’s psychological adjustment.

Results

Descriptive overview of the studies

Table 2 is organized to include the studies by the first author, year of publication and country of sample recruitment. The study design, ART type, family configuration and main outcomes of the mentioned papers are also summarized. In the results section, the included studies will be referenced as SN and the respective study number accordingly to Table 2 (extended version on appendix C available in the OSF, at https://osf.io/pjnbk/?view_only=92979f986dfc419297eed6a8847cda1e).

Table 2 Summary of the studies on the psychological adjustment of children born through ART

Publications started in 1996 with an average of at least one publication per year until 2021, although four particular years summed 16 publications (2007, 2018, 2020 and 2021). The studies’ samples were recruited in eight different countries, with the overwhelming majority of these conducted in the UK (n = 16) and/or the USA (n = 15), followed by The Netherlands (n = 11), Italy (n = 6), Spain (n = 3), France (n = 2), Israel (n = 1) and Taiwan (n = 1). Studies’ sample sizes ranged from 30 to 769 and the average sample size was 153. Most participants were recruited through either probabilistic national samples or through non-probabilistic sampling (e.g., fertility clinics, hospitals, advertisements in targeted publications, public events, through specialists (snowball) and interest groups). The majority of the studies were cross-sectional (n = 26) and 19 studies used prospective longitudinal and/or follow-up research designs. Since some studies are part of the same prospective longitudinal and/or follow-up research, 38 different samples from a total of 45 studies were included in this review. Nineteen studies used a quantitative approach, and 26 studies used a mixed-methods approach (17 quantitative and qualitative studies, 8 quantitative, qualitative and observational studies and 1 quantitative and observational study). Most of the studies used a multi-informant approach (n = 27) including parents, children and teachers (n = 11 studies), parents and children (n = 11 studies), or parents and teachers (5 studies). The remaining studies were conducted solely with parents (17 studies), and 1 study was conducted solely with children.

Psychological adjustment was differently conceptualized in the 45 papers, namely, as socioemotional development/functioning, social development/adjustment/competence, emotional development/adjustment/problems/difficulties, behavioural adjustment/problems, psychological problems/adjustment/well-being. To assess children’s psychological adjustment, the most widely used instruments were the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) forms [CBCL/6–18, TRF] (18 studies) and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire [SDQ] (22 studies) within a multi-informant approach. Only two studies used the ASEBA and SDQ children’s self-report forms. Most of these studies used the ASEBA’s Total Problem, Internalizing and Externalizing Scales or the SDQ’s Total Difficulties Scales. Only one study [29] used the Internalizing and Externalizing Scales. Almost half of these studies compared their samples’ scores with normative data, whereas the remaining studies compared their samples’ scores with different comparison groups (naturally conceived children or adoptive children). Very few studies (n = 6) used the Rutter scales, the Pictorial Scale of Perceived Competence, the Eyberg Child Behaviour Inventory (ECBI), the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale, and the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS) to examine children’s psychological adjustment.

Psychological adjustment of children born through ART

ART children and normative data

The first specific objective of this review was to assess ART children’s psychological adjustment from different cultural contexts/countries and rated by multi-informants according to the normative data for the standardized indexes of mental health. As such, the mean scores of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ [30];) and ASEBA forms (CBCL and TRF [31];) of the children of the included studies were used to perform a comparative analysis with the standardized normative data for those measures (see Appendix B available in the OSF, at https://osf.io/pjnbk/?view_only=92979f986dfc419297eed6a8847cda1e). For the SDQ data interpretation, we followed the original three-band categorization (normal, borderline or abnormal), and for the ASEBA forms the categorization was normal range, borderline clinical range and clinical range. By conducting this analysis, we not only deepen the information that can be drawn from these studies, but also reduce the risk of bias associated with sampling processes, samples’ characteristics and the heterogeneity of the studies’ methodologies. We found that based on both parents’ and teachers’ ratings, children’s psychological adjustment was within the normal range in all the included studies for the SDQ, total scale scores and internalizing/externalizing scores, and for the ASEBA, total scores and internalizing/externalizing scores were also within the normal range.

ART children and different ART

Several ARTs were included in the reviewed studies, namely, donor insemination (DI) (n = 21), in vitro fertilization (IVF) (n = 17), intracytoplasmic sperm injection (ICSI) (n = 4), intrauterine insemination (IUI) (n = 2), gametes donation [oocyte (n = 1) and spermatozoids (n = 3)], egg donation (n = 7), embryo donation (n = 3) and surrogacy (n = 11). The overwhelming majority of the studies included DI and/or IVF, although some studies (n = 9) have not specified the ART used by the participants.

Given the lack of overall psychological adjustment problems in ART children, we will next examine possible differences according to different ART used, especially ART involving reproductive donation where the child is not genetically related to at least one parent. ART that involve IVF and ICSI could include genetic material from both parents, only one or none. However, for most of the articles with these techniques, it was not possible to discriminate if those families included gamete donation or not. This information should be considered in the following results description.

The vast majority of the studies included DI families in their samples and reported that children were well adjusted. These studies found no significant differences between NC and adoptive children on any of the measures of psychological adjustment [SN1; SN4; SN6; SN9; SN13; SN15; SN21; SN22; SN26]. Nevertheless, for two studies conducted in the 1990s, DI children from heterosexual-parented families presented higher levels of emotional problems than NC, IVF and DI children from lesbian-parented families [SN2; SN3]. From a cross-cultural perspective, Dutch DI children reported higher levels of emotional and behavioural problems than children from the UK, Italy and Spain [SN1], whereas in another study, Dutch DI children presented lower levels of emotional and behavioural problems comparatively to the American children [SN18].

Children born through egg and/or embryo donation also presented good social and emotional development overall [SN6; SN13; SN21; SN22; SN23; SN26; SN36; SN39; SN45] and fewer behavioural (conduct) problems than adoptive children [SN14]. Three studies reported no differences between surrogacy children and NC children during their early school years [SN13; SN21; SN23], although one study reported higher levels of adjustment difficulties in 7-year-old surrogacy children, compared to egg donation, DI and NC children [SN26].

The studies revealed that IVF children presented normal psychological adjustment [SN1; SN3; SN5; SN6; SN7; SN8; SN9; SN10; SN14; SN17; SN21; SN27; SN29; SN35; SN38; SN42; SN45], with fewer behaviour problems in one study [SN7], compared to NC children. The large majority of the studies did not discriminate between children born through IVF with gametes from both parents (homologous IVF) and children born through IVF with gametes or embryo donation. Nevertheless, four studies included children born through homologous IVF [SN7; SN8; SN14; SN42;] and one discriminated the different types of IVF and concluded that children’s psychological adjustment was similar regardless of their genetic or gestational link to their parents, nor do they appear to be at higher risk of psychological adjustment problems in middle childhood [SN21].

ART children and different family configurations

The third specific objective was to examine possible differences in children’s psychological adjustment across different family configurations. The majority of the children from single mother or single father families presented good psychological adjustment and high social competences, with no reported significant differences compared to ART children from two-parent families [SN4; SN32; SN38; SN40; SN41; SN42; SN43]. A study in Taiwan that only included single mothers of IVF children reported that they presented fewer behavioural problems than NC children. In one study, children from single mother families presented higher levels of externalizing problems then children from two-parent families [SN24].

Concerning ART children’s psychological adjustment in lesbian- and gay-parented families, the overwhelming majority of the studies found no significant differences between these children and children from heterosexual-parented families in emotional and behavioural difficulties [SN2; SN4; SN16; SN28; SN31; SN33; SN38; SN42]. Further, children from lesbian-/gay-parented families showed no differences in the prevalence of internalizing or externalizing problems when compared to NC children and/or adoptive children [SN11; SN16; SN28; SN30; SN31; SN42; SN44] or normative samples [SN2; SN4; SN20; SN33; SN38]. In fact, some studies revealed that children’s internalizing and externalizing problems from gay-/lesbian-parented families were very low when compared to the cutoff point for clinical problems [SN34; SN36; SN37]. Noteworthy, in two studies, surrogacy children with gay fathers compared to normative samples presented the lowest internalizing problems [SN36], especially the daughters [SN37]. This seems to indicate that there could exist unique family processes in gay father’s surrogacy families.

Two studies concerning ART children from lesbian mothers assessed the influence of the donor status (anonymous or known) in children’s psychological adjustment and found no significance between the two groups [SN15; SN44]. In two other studies, ART children from lesbian/gay single parents presented high levels of self-worth and low levels of internalizing and externalizing problems, and showed no significant differences when compared to ART children from lesbian/gay two-parent families, and heterosexual single- or two-parent families [SN32; SN42]. Nevertheless, in one study with lesbian- and heterosexual-parented families, children from single mother families reported higher levels of externalizing problems when compared to children from two-parent families, regardless of mother’s sexual orientation [SN24].

Only one study with ART children with transgender parents was found and included in this review. The authors used the SDQ to evaluate children’s psychological adjustment and reported that children with transgender parents scored within the normal range for the total difficulties scale. Further, children from both transgender and cisgender-parented families presented high-quality relationships with their parents and good psychological adjustment. Child age at which the transgender parent disclosed their gender identity to their child was not associated with their child’s psychological outcomes [SN41].

Discussion

The present systematic review aimed to assess the psychological adjustment of ART children aged between 3 and 11 years. The specific objectives were the following: (a) to compare ART children’s scores on standardized indexes of mental health with normative data; (b) to examine possible differences on children’s psychological adjustment across different ART especially those involving reproductive donation; (c) and to examine possible differences in children’s psychological adjustment across different family configurations, namely between children from single mother/father families versus coupled parents’ families, lesbian-parented families and/or gay-parented families versus heterosexual-parented families and transgender-parented families versus cisgender-parented families.

Overall, there were no substantial differences between ART children and NC or adoptive children, or differences for internalizing and externalizing problems when ART children were compared with children from normative samples. Almost all studies included in this review have used the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) or the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA) within a multi-informant approach which enables the assessment of children’s psychological adjustment through the standardized indexes provided by these worldwide recognized instruments. We confirmed that similarly to natural conceived children, all ART children examined with the aforementioned measures were under the clinical range for psychiatric disorders.

Further, most of the studies have also used a multi-methods approach which helped to complement the information provided by the questionnaires, since they are limited to capture the respondents’ perspectives on particular questions that are presented [32]. Looking specifically at the SDQ, only one study [33] used the three subscales versions (Internalizing, Externalizing and Prosocial Behaviours), whereas the other studies used the five subscales version (Peer Problems, Emotional Problems, Behaviour Problems, Conduct Problems and Prosocial Behaviours). This particular aspect of the SDQ should be taken into future consideration, since the three versions could be a more suitable and accurate approach to compare multi-informants across the non-clinical population, as mentioned by the scale creators [30] and other authors (e.g., Costa et al. [34]).

Another goal was to examine possible differences on children’s psychological adjustment according to different ART, mainly the techniques that involve gametes and/or egg donation. For most of the studies, no statistically significant differences were found for main indexes of emotional and behavioural problems. Nevertheless, two studies [35, 36] conducted in the 1990s reported some differences between DI children and other ART children and concluded that the differences lean on the DI heterosexual-parented families who reported higher adjustment difficulties compared to DI children from lesbian-parented families and other comparison groups. In these studies, DI heterosexual families were more secretive than IVF families and lesbian-parented families. One possible explanation is the possible influence of the confidentiality and the taboo surrounding male infertility on children’ emotional and behavioural adjustment. Nonetheless, this hypothesis was not verified in other studies included in this review. Another study that did not corroborate the absence of differences between children from different ARTs was a study performed with heterosexual-parented families that have pursued surrogacy and reported higher psychological adjustment problems in surrogacy children when compared to other ART techniques at the age of 7 years [37]. The authors have raised the hypothesis that the absence of a gestational connection between parents and their child could be more problematic for children than the absence of a genetic relationship; however, this difference was not verified when the children were 10 years old. Also, this difference regarding the adjustment was not indicative of psychological disorder, since the surrogacy children SDQ scores were within the normal range. Possibly, this could be related to the time that children begun to ask questions about their origins, since by the age of 6–8 years children start understanding the significance of the biological concept of family and the repercussions of a non-biological connection between family members [38].

Differently from the results, other studies with lesbian and gay-parented families concluded that children from surrogacy gay fathers not only were well adjusted as other ART children or comparison groups, as they were the group with the lowest psychological adjustment problems when compared with normative children [33, 39, 41]. For one particular study, it was the girls who presented the lowest levels of adjustment problems [41]. Similar results were observed in children from families with adoptive gay fathers. These children presented less internalizing/externalizing behaviours, and less gender-stereotyping comparatively with children from heterosexual adoptive families [41, 42]. A recent meta-analysis that included studies mostly with surrogacy gay also found that children of gay fathers may present less internalizing and externalizing problems than children of heterosexual parents and highlighted that these differences could be assigned to parents’ specific sociodemographic characteristic such as higher income and higher education level, and also to an increased resilience and with more egalitarian parenting roles [23]. The degendered parenting associated with some gay fathers, as mentioned in Carneiro and colleagues’ (2017) systematic review, could also be promote these findings [22]. It seems important that future studies rather than just focusing on the different ART and their impact on children’s psychological adjustment could expand the evaluation and assess how the families manage the disclosure and the absence of genetic and/or gestational link with their children. The mentioned differences should be further analysed and contextualized with other family specificities (e.g., family communication, parental style, quality of parental–children relationship).

Other purpose of the present review was to assess whether children from different family configurations present differences on their psychological adjustment. Children from the included studies seem to be well adjusted regardless of their parents’ sexual orientation or number of family members. Heterosexual coupled-parented families return to this technology most likely due to the infertility condition of at least one of the couple members, which is very different from the reason why lesbian, gay and heterosexual single parents pursue this path. In the present study, ART children from lesbian- and gay-parented families do not seem to differ from other comparison groups, nor from children from normative samples for the main psychological adjustment indexes. In fact, for some studies children from lesbian/gay-parented families presented the lowest levels of adjustment problems when compared with children from normative samples [15, 28, 40]. Almost all the studies with single-parent families concluded singlehood does not negatively influence ART children’s psychological adjustment, except for one study with Israeli mothers where children from single-mother families reported more psychological adjustment difficulties than children from coupled mother families regardless of their sexual orientation [43]. Interestingly, a study in Taiwan performed only with single mothers of IVF children concluded that they presented fewer behavioural problems than NC children. The differences mentioned could be because of cultural differences between Western and Eastern countries that should be further investigated.

Although there are several studies assessing the influence of parent’s sexual orientation and ART children’s psychological adjustment, only one study was found with the purpose of evaluating the influence of the gender identity on ART children’s psychological adjustment. This study [39] concluded that children from transgender-parented families were well adjusted with high-quality relationships with their parents. The empirical literature on children’s developmental outcomes on transgender-parented families remains very limited. Due to the increasing awareness of trans equality issues in some countries, these families will become more numerous and noticeable in the coming years [44]; therefore, future studies should conduct more studies with families with trans parents and also adopting sophisticated methodological approaches to understand this new family configuration [1].

There are some limitations arising from this study that should be take into account while interpreting the findings of this review. The retention rates were not included and discriminated in the outcome analysis, nor the levels of agreement of teachers and parents about children’s psychological adjustment. Another limitation of the findings of this review is that we did not include specifically bisexual parents and it is an important and scarce topic in the literature field. Futures studies should focus on these families. As mentioned earlier, studies with transgender-parented families should be conducted to evaluate the children’s well-being and to introduce more information about this family configuration. Also, future studies should address the disclosure issues and understand its possible impact on children’s psychological adjustment. Future studies should also compare the disclosure outcomes according to children’s age to verify if there are significant differences or a more appropriate age to disclose. The impact of the donor status for the family and children’s adjustment should also be further addressed.

In sum, all these different families arise from different contexts and with different restrictions or limitation to their parenthood pathways and probably this could influence children’s psychological adjustment differently but not necessarily negatively. Those idiosyncratic differences will require different psychological resources and strategies to navigate throughout those pathways, which will impact their further decisions to disclose to their children, family members, friends, and acquaintances. The families need to find out what narrative and what strategies would be more suitable for them to maintain or promote good family functioning. This should be taken into account by family practitioners and clinicians to help parents mitigate the impact of these potential stressors and to be more psychologically adjusted to parenthood, and also help them to minimize the impact of these potential stressors, since there are other inevitable stressors inherent to parenthood that are present in all families. The results of this systematic review enable practitioners to develop an informed view of ART children’s psychological adjustment. Also, despite the multiplicity of factors that influence ART families and their children’s psychological health, these results highlight how ART children from different family configurations (heterosexual/lesbian/gay/transexual parents or coupled/single parent(s)) are all similarly psychologically adjusted and without the presence of clinical problems.

List of the references of the included studies

SN1:

Golombok S, Brewaeys A, Cook R et al. (1996) The European study of assisted reproduction families: family functioning and child development. Hum Reprod 11(10):2324–2331. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.humrep.a019098

SN2:

Brewaeys A, Ponjaert I, Van Hall EV, et al (1997a) Donor insemination: child development and family functioning in lesbian mother families. Hum Reprod 12(6):1349-1359. https://doi.org/10.1093/humrep/12.6.1349

SN3:

Brewaeys A, Golombok S, Naaktgeboren N, et al (1997b) Donor insemination: Dutch parents’ opinions about confidentiality and donor anonymity and the emotional adjustment of their children. Hum Reprod 12(7):1591-1597. https://doi.org/10.1093/humrep/12.7.1591.

SN4:

Chan RW, Raboy B, Patterson CJ (1998) Psychosocial adjustment among children conceived via donor insemination by lesbian and heterosexual mothers. Child Dev 69(2):443-457. https://doi.org/10.2307/1132177.

SN5:

Montgomery TR, Aiello F, Adelman RD, et al (1999) The psychological status at school age of children conceived by in-vitro fertilization. Hum Reprod 14(8):2162-2165. https://doi.org/10.1093/humrep/14.8.2162.

SN6:

Golombok S, Murray C (1999) Social versus biological parenting: Family functioning and the socioemotional development of children conceived by egg or sperm donation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 40(4):519-527. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00470.

SN7:

Hahn CS, DiPietro JA (2001) In vitro fertilization and the family: quality of parenting, family functioning, and child psychosocial adjustment. Dev Psychol 37(1):37-48.

SN8:

Golombok S, MacCallum F, Goodman E (2001) The “test‐tube” generation: Parent–child relationships and the psychological well‐being of in vitro fertilization children at adolescence. Child Dev 72(2):599-608. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00299

SN9:

Golombok S, Brewaeys A, Giavazzi MT, et al (2002) The European study of assisted reproduction families: the transition to adolescence. Hum Reprod 17(3):830-840. https://doi.org/10.1093/humrep/17.3.830

SN10:

Tully LA, Moffitt TE, Caspi A (2003) Maternal adjustment, parenting and child behaviour in families of school‐aged twins conceived after IVF and ovulation induction. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44(3):316-325. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00124.

SN11:

Bos HM, Van Balen F, Van Den Boom DC, et al (2004) Minority stress, experience of parenthood and child adjustment in lesbian families. J Reprod Infant Psychol 22(4):291-304. https://doi.org/10.1080/02646830412331298350.

SN12:

Lycett E, Daniels K, Curson R, et al (2004) Offspring created as a result of donor insemination: a study of family relationships, child adjustment, and disclosure. Fertil Steril 82(1):172-179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fertnstert.2003.11.039

SN13:

Golombok S, Murray C, Jadva V, et al (2006) Non-genetic and non-gestational parenthood: consequences for parent–child relationships and the psychological well-being of mothers, fathers and children at age 3. Hum Reprod 21(7):1918-1924. https://doi.org/10.1093/humrep/del039

SN14:

MacCallum F, Golombok S, Brinsden P (2007) Parenting and child development in families with a child conceived through embryo donation. J Fam Psychol 21(2):278-287. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.21.2.278

SN15:

Bos HM, Hakvoort EM (2007) Child adjustment and parenting in planned lesbian families with known and as-yet unknown donors. J Psychosom Obstet Gyneacol 28(2):121-129. https://doi.org/10.1080/01674820701409793.

SN16:

Bos HM, Van Balen F, Van Den Boom DC (2007) Child adjustment and parenting in planned lesbian‐parent families. AM J Orthopsychiatry 77(1):38-48. https://doi.org/10.1037/0002-9432.77.1.38.

SN17:

Golombok S, Olivennes F, Ramogida C, et al (2007) Parenting and the psychological development of a representative sample of triplets conceived by assisted reproduction. Hum Reprod 22(11):2896-2902. https://doi.org/10.1093/humrep/dem260.

SN18:

Bos HMW, Gartrell NK, van Balen F, et al (2008) Children in planned lesbian families: A cross-cultural comparison between the united states and the netherlands. Am J Orthopsychiatry 78(2):211-219. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012711.

SN19:

Bos HMW, Van Balen F (2008) Children in planned lesbian families: Stigmatisation, psychological adjustment and protective factors. Cult Health Sex 10(3):221-236. https://doi.org/10.1080/13691050701601702.

SN20:

Bos HMW, Gartrell NK, Peyser H, et al (2008) The USA national longitudinal lesbian family study (NLLFS): Homophobia, psychological adjustment, and protective factors. J Lesbian Stud 12(4):455-471. https://doi.org/10.1080/10894160802278630.

SN21:

Shelton KH, Boivin J, Hay D, et al (2009) Examining differences in psychological adjustment problems among children conceived by assisted reproductive technologies. Int J Behav Dev 33(5):385-392. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025409338444.

SN22:

Golombok S, Readings J, Blake L, et al (2011) Children conceived by gamete donation: psychological adjustment and mother–child relationships at age 7. J Fam Psychol 25(2):230-9. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022769.

SN23:

Golombok S, Readings J, Blake L, et al (2011) Families created through surrogacy: Mother-child relationships and children’s psychological adjustment at age 7. Dev Psychol 47(6):1579–1588. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025292.

SN24:

Shechner T, Slone M, Lobel TE, et al (2013) Children's adjustment in non‐traditional families in Israel: The effect of parental sexual orientation and the number of parents on children's development. Child Care Health Dev 39(2):178-184. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2011.01337.x.

SN25:

Freeman T, Golombok S (2012) Donor insemination: a follow-up study of disclosure decisions, family relationships and child adjustment at adolescence. Reprod BioMed Online 25(2):193-203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rbmo.2012.03.009.

SN26:

Golombok S, Blake L, Casey P, et al (2013) Children born through reproductive donation: a longitudinal study of psychological adjustment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 54(6):653-660. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12015.

SN27:

Anderson KN, Koh BD, Connor JJ, et al (2014) Twins conceived using assisted reproduction: parent mental health, family relationships and child adjustment at middle childhood. Hum Reprod 29(10):2247-2255. https://doi.org/10.1093/humrep/deu190.

SN28:

Baiocco R, Santamaria F, Ioverno S, et al (2015) Lesbian mother families and gay father families in Italy: Family functioning, dyadic satisfaction, and child well-being. Sex Res Social Policy 12(3):202-212. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13178-015-0185-x.

SN29:

Anderson KN, Rueter MA, Connor JJ, et al (2015) Conformity expectations: Differential effects on IVF twins and singletons’ parent–child relationships and adjustment. J Fam Psychol 29(4):558-567. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000122.

SN30:

Tornello SL, Sonnenberg BN, Patterson CJ (2015) Division of labor among gay fathers: Associations with parent, couple, and child adjustment. Psychol Sex Orientat Gend Divers 2(4):365-375. https://doi.org/10.1037/SGD0000109.

SN31:

Bos HMW, Knox J, van Rijn-van Gelderen L, et al (2016) Same-sex and different-sex parent households and child health outcomes: Findings from the National Survey of Children’s Health. J Dev Behav Pediatr 37(3):179-87. https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000288.

SN32:

Golombok S, Zadeh S, Imrie S, et al (2016) Single mothers by choice: Mother–child relationships and children’s psychological adjustment. J Fam Psychol 30(4):409-418. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000188.

SN33:

Bos HMW, Kuyper L, Gartrell NK (2018) A population‐based comparison of female and male same‐sex parent and different‐sex parent households. Fam Process 57(1):148-164. https://doi.org/10.1111/famp.12278.

SN34:

Golombok S, Blake L, Slutsky J, et al (2018) Parenting and the adjustment of children born to gay fathers through surrogacy. Child Dev 89(4):1223-1233. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12728.

SN35:

Anderson KN, Rueter MA, Connor JJ, et al (2019) Parental conformity expectations' effect on twins' and singletons' parent–adolescent relationships: Associations with change in adjustment from middle childhood to adolescence. J Res Adolesc 29(4):832-845. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12416.

SN36:

Carone N, Lingiardi V, Chirumbolo A, et al (2018) Italian gay father families formed by surrogacy: Parenting, stigmatization, and children’s psychological adjustment. Dev Psychol 54(10):1904-1916. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000571.

SN37:

Green RJ, Rubio RJ, Rothblum ED, et al (2019) Gay fathers by surrogacy: prejudice, parenting, and well-being of female and male children. Psychol Sex Orientat Gend Divers 6(3):269-283. https://doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000325.

SN38:

Carone N, Baiocco R, Lingiardi V, et al (2020) Gay and heterosexual single father families created by surrogacy: Father–child relationships, parenting quality, and children’s psychological adjustment. Sex Res Social Policy 17(4):711-728. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13178-019-00428-7.

SN39:

Chen M, Rueter MA, Anderson KN, et al (2020) Conversation orientation moderates the relationship between information sharing of medically assisted reproduction and child adjustment. Fam Process 59(1):229-243. https://doi.org/10.1111/famp.12415.

SN40:

Golombok S, Zadeh S, Freeman T, et al (2020) Single mothers by choice: Parenting and child adjustment in middle childhood. J Fam Psychol 35(2):192-202. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000797.

SN41:

Imrie S, Golombok S (2020) Impact of new family forms on parenting and child development. Annu Rev Dev Psychol 2:295–316. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-070220-122704.

SN42:

Carone N, Barone L, Lingiardi V, et al (2021) Factors associated with behavioral adjustment among school-age children of gay and heterosexual single fathers through surrogacy. Dev Psychol 57(4):535-547. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001155.

SN43:

Díez M, González M, Morgado B (2021) Single mothers by choice in Spain: Parenting and psychosocial adjustment in adopted and ART children. J Fam Psychol 35(6):767–779. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000680.

SN44:

Carone N, Gartrell NK, Rothblum ED, et al (2021) The stability of psychological adjustment among donor-conceived offspring in the US National Longitudinal Lesbian Family Study from childhood to adulthood: Differences by donor type. Fertil Steril 115(5):1302-1311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fertnstert.2020.12.012.

SN45:

Gervoise-Boyer MJ, Anzola AB, Sambuc R, et al (2021) Health and well-being outcomes of adolescents conceived through in vitro fertilization and intracytoplasmic sperm injection. Reprod Sci 28(5):1428-1438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s43032-020-00407-z.