Skip to main content
Log in

Maternal warmth and toddler development: support for transactional models in disadvantaged families

  • Original Contribution
  • Published:
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Studies support cognitive and social domains of development as entwined in childhood, however, there is a paucity of investigation into the nature of the mother–child relationship within an interdependence framework. Furthermore, the focus on these processes within families from impoverished communities using frequent assessments in early childhood has been limited. Our objectives were to identify (1) the directional associations between toddler’s communication ability and social competence, (2) to establish whether the association between toddler’s communication ability and social competence is mediated by maternal warmth, and (3) to establish support for transactional models between toddlers’ outcomes and maternal warmth in disadvantaged communities in Ireland. Participants included 173 toddlers and their families enrolled in a prenatally commencing prevention programme. Toddler’s communication and social competence were assessed at 12, 18, 24 and 36 months and maternal warmth at 6 and 24 months. Cross-lagged models were estimated examining multiple paths of associations simultaneously. Direct and indirect paths of maternal warmth were also examined. Bi-directional associations were found between communication ability and social competence from 12 to 24 months but not thereafter. Maternal warmth did not significantly mediate these associations, however, support of a transactional model was found with social competence. The results support early positive associations between better communication ability and social competence in the first 2 years, however, they suggest that these associations are no longer present by the third year. The role of maternal warmth in fostering social competencies is important for toddlers and equally important is toddler’s level of social competence in eliciting increased maternal warmth.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Aunola K, Nurmi JE (2005) The role of parenting styles in children’s problem behavior. Child Dev 76(6):1144–1159

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Barnett MA, Gustafsson H, Deng M, Mills-Koonce WR, Cox M (2012) Bidirectional associations among sensitive parenting, language development, and social competence. Infant Child Dev 21(4):374–393

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Fraley RC, Roisman GI, Haltigan JD (2013) The legacy of early experiences in development: formalizing alternative models of how early experiences are carried forward over time. Dev Psychol 49(1):109

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Knafo A, Plomin R (2006) Parental discipline and affection and children’s prosocial behavior: genetic and environmental links. J Pers Soc Psychol 90(1):147

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Pungello EP, Iruka IU, Dotterer AM, Mills-Koonce R, Reznick JS (2009) The effects of socioeconomic status, race, and parenting on language development in early childhood. Dev Psychol 45(2):544

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Wang F, Christ SL, Mills-Koonce WR, Garrett-Peters P, Cox MJ (2013) Association between maternal sensitivity and externalizing behavior from preschool to preadolescence. J Appl Dev Psychol 34(2):89–100

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Weiss B, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS (1992) Some consequences of early harsh discipline: child aggression and a maladaptive social information processing style. Child Dev 63(6):1321–1335

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Cochran MM, Brassard JA (1979) Child development and personal social networks. Child Dev 1:601–616

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Vygotsky LS (1986) Thought and language (rev. ed.). MIT Press, Cambridge

  10. Daniel E, Madigan S, Jenkins J (2015) Paternal and maternal warmth motivates preschoolers development of prosociality. J Fam Psychol Early View. doi:10.1037/fam0000120

    Google Scholar 

  11. Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR, Miller-Loncar CL (2000) Early maternal and child influences on children’s later independent cognitive and social functioning. Child Dev 71(2):358–375

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Miller-Loncar CL, Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR (2000) The influence of complexity of maternal thoughts on sensitive parenting and children’s social responsiveness. J Appl Dev Psychol 21(3):335–356

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Smith KE, Landry SH, Swank PR (2000) Does the content of mothers’ verbal stimulation explain differences in children’s development of verbal and nonverbal cognitive skills? J Sch Psychol 38(1):27–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Combs-Ronto LA, Olson SL, Lunkenheimer ES, Sameroff AJ (2009) Interactions between maternal parenting and children’s early disruptive behavior: bidirectional associations across the transition from preschool to school entry. J Abnorm Child Psychol 37(8):1151–1163

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Smith CL, Calkins SD, Keane SP, Anastopoulos AD, Shelton TL (2004) Predicting stability and change in toddler behavior problems: contributions of maternal behavior and child gender. Dev Psychol 40(1):29

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kelley HH, Holmes JG, Kerr NL, Reis HT, Rusbult CE, Van Lange PA (2003) An atlas of interpersonal situations. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  17. Kuczynski L, Mitchell B (2009) Dialectics and transactional modes: conceptualizing antecedents, processes, and consequences of change in parent–child relationships. In: Roberto KA, Mancini JA (eds) Pathways of human development: explorations of change. Lexington Books, Lanham, pp 107–151

    Google Scholar 

  18. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF (2002) Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol 53(1):371–399

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Bard KA (1992) Intentional behavior and intentional communication in young free-ranging orangutans. Child Dev 63(5):1186–1197

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Hoff E (2006) Language experience and language milestones during early childhood. In: McCartney K, Philips D (eds) Blackwell handbook of early childhood development. Blackwell, Malden, MA, pp 232–251

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  21. Mundy P, Gomes A (1998) Individual differences in joint attention skill development in the second year. Infant Behav Dev 21(3):469–482

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Mundy P, Block J, Delgado C, Pomares Y, Van Hecke AV, Parlade MV (2007) Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Dev 78(3):938–954

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Fabes RA, Gaertner BM, Popp TK (2006) Getting along with others: social competence in early childhood. In: McCartney K, Philips D (eds) Blackwell handbook of early childhood development. Blackwell, Malden, MA, pp 296–316

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  24. Rhee SH, Boeldt DL, Friedman NP, Corley RP, Hewitt JK, Young SE, Knafo A, Robinson J, Waldman ID, Van Hulle CA, Zahn-Waxler C (2013) The role of language in concern and disregard for others in the first years of life. Dev Psychol 49(2):197

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Doyle O, Fitzpatrick N, Lovett J, Rawdon C (2015) Early intervention and child physical health: evidence from a Dublin-based randomized controlled trial. Econ Hum Biol 19:224–245

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Doyle O (2013) Breaking the cycle of deprivation: an experimental evaluation of an early childhood intervention. J Stat Soc Inquiry Soc Irel; XLI 92:111

    Google Scholar 

  27. Bricker D, Squires J, Mounts L, Potter L, Nickel R, Twombly E, Farrell J (1999) Ages and stages questionnaires. Paul H. Brookes, Baltimore

    Google Scholar 

  28. Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS (2006) BITSEA: Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment. Examiner’s manual. Harcourt Assessment, San Antonio

    Google Scholar 

  29. Boivin M, Pérusse D, Dionne G, Saysset V, Zoccolillo M, Tarabulsy GM, Tremblay N, Tremblay RE (2005) The genetic-environmental etiology of parents’ perceptions and self-assessed behaviours toward their 5-month-old infants in a large twin and singleton sample. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46(6):612–630

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Little RJ, Rubin DB (2014) Statistical analysis with missing data. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  31. Abidin RR (1990) Parenting stress index-short form. Pediatric Psychology Press, Charlottesville

    Google Scholar 

  32. Selig JP, Little TD (2012) Autoregressive and cross-lagged panel analysis for longitudinal data. In: Laursen B, Little TD, Card NA (eds) Handbook of developmental research methods. The Guilford Press, New York, pp 265–278

    Google Scholar 

  33. Muthén LK, Muthén BO (1998–2015) Mplus user’s guide, 7th edn. Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles

  34. Browne MW, Cudeck R (1993) Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sage Focus Editions, Newbury Park, pp 154, 136

  35. Bentler PM (1990) Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull 107(2):238

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Diamantopoulos A, Siguaw JA (2013) Introducing LISREL: a guide for the uninitiated. SAGE, London

    Google Scholar 

  37. Hu LT, Bentler PM (1999) Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model: Multidiscip J 6(1):1–55

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Gallagher TM (1993) Language skill and the development of social competence in school-age children. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch 24(4):199–205

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Marshall HM, Hightower AD, Fritton JE, Russell RS, Meller PJ (1996) Enhance social competence program. Primary Mental Health Project Inc., Rochester

    Google Scholar 

  40. Manolson A (1992) It takes two to talk: za parent’s guide to helping children communicate. The Hanen Centre, Toronto

    Google Scholar 

  41. Girard LC, Pingault JB, Doyle O, Falissard B, Tremblay RE (2016) Expressive language and prosocial behaviour in early childhood: Longitudinal associations in the UK Millennium Cohort Study. Eur J Dev Psychol. doi:10.1080/17405629.2016.1215300

  42. Schmitt N (1996) Uses and abuses of coefficient alpha. Psychol Assess 8(4):350–353

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Squires JK, Nickel R, Bricker D (1990) Use of parent-completed developmental questionnaires for child-find and screening. Infants Young Child 3(2):46–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Dr. Girard is supported by a Marie Curie International Incoming Fellowship; the research leading to these results has received funding from the People Programme (Marie Curie Actions) of the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme FP7/2007-2013/under REA Grant Agreement No. [625014]. The data used in the current study was collected as part of the evaluation of the Preparing for Life programme, which was funded by the Northside Partnership through the Atlantic Philanthropies and the Irish Government’s Department of Children and Youth Affairs.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisa-Christine Girard.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Appendix 1

Appendix 1

See Fig. 3.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Sensitivity analysis. Cross-lagged model examining both direct and indirect paths of maternal warmth at 6 and 24 months (denoted as warmth) in the longitudinal bi-directional associations between communication ability (denoted by communicate) and social competence (denoted by competence) between 12 and 36 months (N = 142). Standard errors presented within brackets. Only significant paths are presented for visual simplicity. Treatment status (denoted by group) was controlled in the model but was not statistically significant

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Girard, LC., Doyle, O. & Tremblay, R.E. Maternal warmth and toddler development: support for transactional models in disadvantaged families. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 26, 497–507 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-016-0913-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-016-0913-7

Keywords

Navigation