Skip to main content
Log in

Lifetime familiarity cue effects for autobiographical memory

  • Research
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recollecting an autobiographical memory requires a cue to initiate processes related to accessing and then elaborating on a past personal experience. Prior work has shown that the familiarity of a cue can influence the autobiographical memory retrieval process. Extending this work, we tested how familiarity accrued from cumulative lifetime exposures associated with the cue—as well as associated semantic knowledge—can affect how we access and remember autobiographical memories. In Experiment 1, we measured reaction times to access and report memories in response to cue words. In Experiment 2 we examined the details with which participants described memories in response to cue words. For both experiments, participants provided estimates of lifetime exposure and semantic knowledge for each cue. In Experiment 1, we found a cue’s lifetime exposure, independent of semantic knowledge, led to quicker memory access. In Experiment 2, we found the lifetime exposure and semantic knowledge of a cue interactively affected the specificity of a described autobiographical memory. These results provide new evidence that the amount of lifetime exposure associated with a cue, both independently and interactively with semantic knowledge, influences how autobiographical memories are accessed and described.

Clinical trial This was not a clinical trial.

Trial Registration Number (TRN) N/A.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

All data, analyses, and study materials will be made available upon reasonable request from the corresponding author.

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1974). Retrieval of propositional information from long-term memory. Cognitive Psychology, 6(4), 451–474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., & Reder, L. M. (1999). The fan effect: New results and new theories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(2), 186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, N. D., Baena, E., Yang, H., & Köhler, S. (2021). Deficits in recent but not lifetime familiarity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia, 151, 107735.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armson, M. J., Abdi, H., & Levine, B. (2017). Bridging naturalistic and laboratory assessment of memory: The baycrest mask fit test. Memory (Hove, England), 25(8), 999–1008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, K. M., McDermott, K. B., & Szpunar, K. K. (2011). Imagining the near and far future: The role of location familiarity. Memory & Cognition, 39(6), 954–967.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Juola, J. F. (1974). Search and decision processes in recognition memory. WH Freeman.

  • Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge Social Psychology, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8279.1933.tb02913.x.

  • Barzykowski, K., & Staugaard, S. R. (2016). Does retrieval intentionality really matter? Similarities and differences between involuntary memories and directly and generatively retrieved voluntary memories. British Journal of Psychology, 107(3), 519–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berntsen, D. (1998). Voluntary and involuntary access to autobiographical memory. Memory (Hove, England), 6(2), 113–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M. A. (1990). Associations between autobiographical memories and concepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 16(5), 799.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(4), 594–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M. A., & Bekerian, D. A. (1987). Organization in autobiographical memory. Memory & Cognition, 15(2), 119–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craney, T. A., & Surles, J. G. (2002). Model-dependent variance inflation factor cutoff values. Quality Engineering, 14(3), 391–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crovitz, H. F., & Schiffman, H. (1974). Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4(5), 517–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Argembeau, A., & Van Der Linden, M. (2004). Phenomenal characteristics associated with projecting oneself back into the past and forward into the future: Influence of valence and temporal distance. Consciousness and Cognition, 13(4), 844–858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.07.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duke, D., Martin, C. B., Bowles, B., McRae, K., & Köhler, S. (2017). Perirhinal cortex tracks degree of recent as well as cumulative lifetime experience with object concepts. Cortex; a Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior, 89, 61–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, J. H., Rubin, D. C., & Giovanello, K. S. (2016). The effects of song familiarity and age on phenomenological characteristics and neural recruitment during autobiographical memory retrieval. Psychomusicology: Music Mind and Brain, 26(3), 199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galton, F. (1879). Psychometric experiments. Brain, 2(2), 149–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilboa, A., & Marlatte, H. (2017). Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(8), 618–631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grühn, D. (2016). An English word database of emotional terms (EMOTE). Psychological Reports, 119(1), 290–308.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haque, S., & Conway, M. A. (2001). Sampling the process of autobiographical memory construction. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13(4), 529–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. B., & Berntsen, D. (2019). Direct and generative autobiographical memory retrieval: How different are they? Consciousness and Cognition, 74, 102793.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. B., & O’Connor, A. R. (2022). On the same wavelength: The impact of other-generated cues on the reported retrieval processes and qualities of autobiographical memories. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.

  • Harris, C. B., O’Connor, A. R., & Sutton, J. (2015). Cue generation and memory construction in direct and generative autobiographical memory retrieval. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 204–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1976). Repetition and memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 10, 47–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, A. J., & Burgess, N. (2014). Pattern completion in multielement event engrams. Current Biology, 24(9), 988–992.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Irish, M., & Piguet, O. (2013). The pivotal role of semantic memory in remembering the past and imagining the future. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, S. F., & Plunkett, K. (1987). Remembering experienced and reported events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 1(1), 15–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenth, R. V. (2001). Some practical guidelines for effective sample size determination. The American Statistician, 55(3), 187–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 677–689. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.4.677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loken, E., & Gelman, A. (2017). Measurement error and the replication crisis. Science, 355(6325), 584–585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, J. H., & Clevinger, A. M. (2013). Priming voluntary autobiographical memories: Implications for the organisation of autobiographical memory and voluntary recall processes. Memory (Hove, England), 21(4), 524–536.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, J. H., McQueen, M. L., Hayslett, K. E., Staley, B. J. A., & Welch, T. J. (2019). Semantic memories prime autobiographical memories: General implications and implications for everyday autobiographical remembering. Memory & Cognition, 47(2), 299–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, J. H., Petersen, E. P., & Kruchten, E. A. (2021a). Elucidating the mental processes underlying the direct retrieval of autobiographical memories. Consciousness and Cognition, 94, 103190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, J. H., Staley, B. J. A., & Sopoci, M. K. (2021b). When trying to recall our past, all roads lead to Rome: More evidence for the multi-process retrieval theory of autobiographical memory. Memory & Cognition, 49(3), 438–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mair, A., Poirier, M., & Conway, M. A. (2021). Age effects in autobiographical memory depend on the measure. PloS One, 16(10), e0259279.

  • McLelland, V. C., Devitt, A. L., Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2015). Making the future memorable: The phenomenology of remembered future events. Memory (Hove, England), 23(8), 1255–1263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McRae, K., Cree, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., & McNorgan, C. (2005). Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things. Behavior Research Methods, 37(4), 547–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno-Martínez, F. J., Montoro, P. R., & Rodríguez-Rojo, I. C. (2014). Spanish norms for age of acquisition, concept familiarity, lexical frequency, manipulability, typicality, and other variables for 820 words from 14 living/nonliving concepts. Behavior Research Methods, 46(4), 1088–1097.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moscovitch, M. (1992). A neuropsychological model of memory and consciousness. Neuropsychology of Memory, 2, 5–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moulin, C. J. A., Carreras, F., & Barzykowski, K. (2022). The phenomenology of autobiographical retrieval. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, e1638.

  • Opitz, B., & Cornell, S. (2006). Contribution of familiarity and recollection to associative recognition memory: Insights from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(9), 1595–1605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reiser, B. J., Black, J. B., & Abelson, R. P. (1985). Knowledge structures in the organization and retrieval of autobiographical memories. Cognitive Psychology, 17(1), 89–137. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(85)90005-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renoult, L., Irish, M., Moscovitch, M., & Rugg, M. D. (2019). From knowing to remembering: The semantic–episodic distinction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(12), 1041–1057.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robin, J., & Moscovitch, M. (2014). The effects of spatial contextual familiarity on remembered scenes, episodic memories, and imagined future events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 40(2), 459.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robin, J., & Moscovitch, M. (2017). Familiar real-world spatial cues provide memory benefits in older and younger adults. Psychology and Aging, 32(3), 210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robin, J., Garzon, L., & Moscovitch, M. (2019). Spontaneous memory retrieval varies based on familiarity with a spatial context. Cognition, 190, 81–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, J. A. (1976). Sampling autobiographical memory. Cognitive Psychology, 8(4), 578–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. C., & Schulkind, M. D. (1997). Properties of word cues for autobiographical memory. Psychological Reports, 81(1), 47–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (2012). Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. American Psychologist. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Norman, K. A., & Koutstaal, W. (1998). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49(1), 289–318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(9), 657–661. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic simulation of future events: Concepts, data, and applications. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 39–60. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1440.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schröder, A., Gemballa, T., Ruppin, S., & Wartenburger, I. (2012). German norms for semantic typicality, age of acquisition, and concept familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 380–394.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar, K. K., & McDermott, K. B. (2008). Episodic future thought and its relation to remembering: Evidence from ratings of subjective experience. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(1), 330–334.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tibon, R., Gronau, N., Scheuplein, A. L., Mecklinger, A., & Levy, D. A. (2014). Associative recognition processes are modulated by the semantic unitizability of memoranda. Brain and Cognition, 92, 19–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treves, A., & Rolls, E. T. (1992). Computational constraints suggest the need for two distinct input systems to the hippocampal CA3 network. Hippocampus, 2(2), 189–199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 26(1), 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Thompson, D. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval process in episodic process. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 353–373.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uzer, T., & Brown, N. R. (2017). The effect of cue content on retrieval from autobiographical memory. Acta Psychologica, 172, 84–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uzer, T., Lee, P. J., & Brown, N. R. (2012). On the prevalence of directly retrieved autobiographical memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 38(5), 1296.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Y., Mao, X., Li, B., Lu, B., & Guo, C. (2016). Semantic memory influences episodic retrieval by increased familiarity. Neuroreport, 27(10), 774–782.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1975). Buildup of proactive inhibition as a cue-overload effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1(4), 442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M., & Broadbent, K. (1986). Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(2), 144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. M. G., Healy, H. G., & Ellis, N. C. (1999). The effect of imageability and predicability of cues in autobiographical memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 52(3), 555–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(3), 441–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Miranda Renda and Elizabeth Dutemple for help with data collection and scoring.

Funding

This study was supported by a NSERC Discovery grant awarded to SS (#RGPIN-04241) and a NSERC Discovery grant awarded to SK (#RGPIN 2018–05770).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

L.G.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Data collection, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing-Reviewing and Editing. H.Y.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing-Reviewing and Editing. S.K.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing-Reviewing and Editing. S.S.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing-Reviewing and Editing.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Signy Sheldon.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

Ethics was reviewed and approved by the McGill University Research Ethics Board (REB # 151–0915).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gurguryan, L., Yang, H., Köhler, S. et al. Lifetime familiarity cue effects for autobiographical memory. Psychological Research (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-01968-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-024-01968-3

Navigation