Skip to main content
Log in

Re-examining the effect of motivation on intentional and unintentional task-unrelated thought: accounting for thought constraint produces novel results

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It has been proposed that motivating participants to perform well on a cognitive task ought to lead to decreases in rates of intentional, but not unintentional, task-unrelated thought (TUT; a commonly studied variety of mind wandering). However, at odds with this prediction, research has found that increasing motivation results in decreases in both intentional and unintentional TUTs. One possible explanation for this surprising finding is that standard assessments of TUT may inadvertently conflate TUTs with another variety of mind wandering: unconstrained thought. If so, then deconfounding task-unrelated and unconstrained varieties of mind wandering might produce the predicted effect of a decrease in intentional, but not unintentional, TUT when motivation is increased. To explore this possibility, in the present study, participants completed a sustained-attention task after receiving standard instructions (normal-motivation condition) or instructions informing them that they could leave the study early if they achieved a certain level of performance (motivated condition). Throughout the task, we assessed rates of TUT (both intentional and unintentional) and unconstrained thoughts. Consistent with prior work, the results indicated that motivated participants reported being on-task significantly more frequently than non-motivated participants. However, unlike previous work, we found that when deconfounding TUTs and unconstrained thoughts, participants in the motivation condition reported significantly fewer bouts of intentional TUT than those in the non-motivation condition, but no differences in rates of unintentional TUT were observed between groups. These results suggest that (a) motivation specifically targets intentional TUT and (b) standard assessments of TUT conflate task-relatedness and thought constraint.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of data and material

Data analysis code and anonymized data will be available on OSF (https://osf.io/3jqcr/) following acceptance.

Code availability

Program code will be available on OSF (https://osf.io/3jqcr/) following acceptance.

Notes

  1. In the Motivated group, to meet the performance criterion for leaving the experiment early, participants merely had to respond (via a spacebar press) to at least one of the 900 MRT tones (Seli et al., 2019). All participants met and surpassed this criterion and were therefore allowed to leave the experiment early (after completing 900 MRT trials). Participants in the Control group were informed, after completing 900 MRT trials, that they could leave the experiment early, irrespective of their performance on the MRT (although all of these participants likewise met and surpassed the criterion that we had set for the Motived group).

  2. Also see O’Neill et al., (2020).

References

  • Anshari, M., Almunawar, M. N., Shahrill, M., Wicaksono, D. K., & Huda, M. (2017). Smartphones usage in the classrooms: Learning aid or interference? Education and Information Technologies, 22(6), 3063–3079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antrobus, J. S., Singer, J. L., & Greenberg, S. (1966). Studies in the stream of consciousness: experimental enhancement and suppression of spontaneous cognitive processes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23(2), 399–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brosowsky, N. P., Smith, A. P., Schooler, J. W., & Seli, P. (2020). The influence of task difficulty on thought constraint. [Paper submitted for publication].

  • Calderwood, C., Ackerman, P. L., & Conklin, E. M. (2014). What else do college students “do” while studying? An investigation of multitasking. Computers & Education, 75, 19–29.

  • Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff, K., Mills, C., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Irving, Z. C., Thompson, E., Fox, K. C., & Kam, J. W. (2018). Mind-wandering as a scientific concept: cutting through the definitional haze. Trends in cognitive sciences, 22(11), 957–959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dienes, Z. (2014). Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant results. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 781. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00781.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J, & Sanford W. (2019). An R companion to applied regression. Third. Thousand Oaks CA: Sage. https://socialsciences.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion/.

  • Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory method for investigating influences on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal.

  • Greer, T., & Dunlap, W. P. (1997). Analysis of variance with ipsative measures. Psychological Methods, 2(2), 200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, D. C. (2012). Statistical methods for psychology. Belmont: Cengage Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeffreys, H. (1961). Theory of probability, 3. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H., Klinger, E., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Spontaneous thought and vulnerability to mood disorders: The dark side of the wandering mind. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(5), 835–857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCoy, B. R. (2016). Digital distractions in the classroom phase II: Student classroom use of digital devices for non-class related purposes (p. 90). Lincoln: Faculty Publications College of Journalism and Mass Communications.

    Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Adrift in the stream of thought: The effects of mind wandering on executive control and working memory capacity. Handbook of individual differences in cognition (pp. 321–334). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: general, 141(2), 302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mills, C., Raffaelli, Q., Irving, Z. C., Stan, D., & Christoff, K. (2018). Is an off-task mind a freely-moving mind? Examining the relationship between different dimensions of thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 58, 20–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morey, R. D., & Rouder, J. N. (2018). BayseFactor: computation of bayes factors for common designs. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=BayesFactor.

  • Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., Franklin, M. S., Chin, J. M., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). The role of mind-wandering in measurements of general aptitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, K., Smith, A. P., Smilek, D., & Seli, P. (2020). Dissociating the freely-moving thought dimension of mind-wandering from the intentionality and task-unrelated thought dimensions. Psychological Research, 1–11.

  • R Core Team. (2019). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.

  • Ralph, B. C. W., Seli, P., Wilson, K. E., & Smilek, D. (2018). Volitional media multitasking: awareness of performance costs and modulation of media multitasking as a function of task demand. Psychological Research, 1–20.

  • Ralph, B. C., Smith, A. C., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2019). The relation between task-unrelated media multitasking and task-related motivation. Psychological Research, 1–15.

  • Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). Oops!': performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747–758.

  • Robison, M. K., & Unsworth, N. (2018). Cognitive and contextual correlates of spontaneous and deliberate mind-wandering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(1), 85.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(2), 225–237. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.2.225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Carriere, J. S., & Smilek, D. (2015a). Not all mind wandering is created equal: Dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 79(5), 750–758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Jonker, T. R., Cheyne, J. A., Cortes, K., & Smilek, D. (2015). Can research participants comment authoritatively on the validity of their self-reports of mind wandering and task engagement? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 703.

  • Seli, P., Carriere, J. S., Thomson, D. R., Cheyne, J. A., Martens, K. A. E., & Smilek, D. (2014). Restless mind, restless body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 660.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Carriere, J. S., Wammes, J. D., Risko, E. F., Schacter, D. L., & Smilek, D. (2018). On the clock: evidence for the rapid and strategic modulation of mind wandering. Psychological Science, 29(8), 1247–1256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering minds and wavering rhythms: Linking mind wandering and behavioral variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(1), 1–5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., Xu, M., Purdon, C., & Smilek, D. (2015b). Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: Implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(5), 1417–1425.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Metzinger, T., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L., Maillet, D., & Smilek, D. (2018c). The family-resemblances framework for mind-wandering remains well clad. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(11), 959–961.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Smallwood, J., Schacter, D. L., Maillet, D., Schooler, J. W., & Smilek, D. (2018b). Mind-wandering as a natural kind: A family-resemblances view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(6), 479–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2016a). On the necessity of distinguishing between unintentional and intentional mind wandering. Psychological Science, 27(5), 685–691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016b). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 605–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Schacter, D. L., Risko, E. F., & Smilek, D. (2019). Increasing participant motivation reduces rates of intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 83(5), 1057–1069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Wammes, J., Risko, E., & Smilek, D. (2016c). On the relation between motivation and retention in educational contexts: The role of intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(4), 1280–1287. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0979-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Westfall, J., & Aust, F. (2019). Afex: Analysis of Factorial Experiments. R package version 0.23–0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afex.

  • Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: the importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 441. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00441.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes, J. (1994). Stimulus-driven capture and attentional set: selective search for color and visual abrupt onsets. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(4), 799.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Theeuwes, J. (2004). Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 65–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turnbull, A., Wang, H. T., Murphy, C., Ho, N. S. P., Wang, X., Sormaz, M., & Vatansever, D. (2019). Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex supports context-dependent prioritisation of off-task thought. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unsworth, N., & McMillan, B. D. (2013). Mind wandering and reading comprehension: Examining the roles of working memory capacity, interest, motivation, and topic experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39(3), 832

  • Vinski, M. T., & Watter, S. (2012). Priming honesty reduces subjective bias in self-report measures of mind wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 451–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wammes, J. D., Boucher, P. O., Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2016). Mind wandering during lectures I: Changes in rates across an entire semester. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 2(1), 13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wammes, J. D., Ralph, B. C. W., Mills, C., Bosch, N., Duncan, T. L., & Smilek, D. (2019). Disengagement during lectures: Media multitasking and mind wandering in university classrooms. Computers & Education, 132, 76–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council Discovery Grant awarded to Daniel Smilek.

Funding

This research was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council Discovery Grant awarded to Daniel Smilek.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alyssa C. Smith.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Smith, A.C., Brosowsky, N.P., Ralph, B.C.W. et al. Re-examining the effect of motivation on intentional and unintentional task-unrelated thought: accounting for thought constraint produces novel results. Psychological Research 86, 87–97 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01487-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-021-01487-5

Navigation