Skip to main content
Log in

Sex differences in hemispheric lateralization of attentional networks

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Males and females differ in various abilities. However, sex differences in hemispheric lateralization of attentional processing are still not well-understood. Using a lateralized version of the attentional network test that combines the Posner cueing paradigm and visual field methodology, we aimed to examine sex differences in the lateralization of several attentional processes including alerting, executive control, orienting benefit, reorienting, and orienting cost. Fifty-six females and 59 males participated in this study. We found a left visual field (right hemisphere) advantage for alerting defined by the differences between no-cue and center-cue conditions in the male group, but it was mainly attributed to the left visual field advantage in the no-cue condition. In contrast, the female group exhibited a left visual field advantage in the center-cue condition. Both groups showed preferences to the left visual field for reorienting and orienting cost, but females exhibited larger effects. This indicates that the two sexes exhibit similarities in terms of the lateralization of these two attentional processes. Furthermore, the interactions between executive control and reorienting/orienting cost were more efficient in males than in females. The current study highlights sex differences in the hemispheric lateralization of attentional networks and possible underlying neural substrates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Asanowicz, D., Marzecova, A., Jaskowski, P., & Wolski, P. (2012). Hemispheric asymmetry in the efficiency of attentional networks. Brain and Cognition, 79(2), 117–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bayliss, A. P., di Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Sex differences in eye gaze and symbolic cueing of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58(4), 631–650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beer, A. L. (2016). Nicotine and cognition: Effects of nicotine on attention and memory systems in humans. In V. R. Preedy (Ed.), Neuropathology of drug addictions and substance misuse (1st ed., pp. 282–290). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological), 57, 289–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruyer, R., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). Combining speed and accuracy in cognitive psychology: Is the Inverse Efficiency Score (IES) a better dependent variable than the mean Reaction Time (RT) and the Percentage of Errors (PE)? Psychologia Belgica, 51(1), 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(6), 215–222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Callejas, A., Lupiáñez, J., & Tudela, P. (2004). The three attentional networks: On their independence and interactions. Brain and cognition, 54(3), 225–227.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christakou, A., Halari, R., Smith, A. B., Ifkovits, E., Brammer, M., & Rubia, K. (2009). Sex-dependent age modulation of frontostriatal and temporo-parietal activation during cognitive control. Neuroimage, 48(1), 223–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clayson, P. E., Clawson, A., & Larson, M. J. (2011). Sex differences in electrophysiological indices of conflict monitoring. Biological Psychology, 87(2), 282–289.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, Z. Z., Gotlieb, N., Erez, O., Wiznitzer, A., Arbel, O., Matas, D., & Henik, A. (2019). Attentional networks during the menstrual cycle. bioRxiv.

  • Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(3), 201–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Grasby, P. M. (1996). A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: A PET study of sustained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia, 34(11), 1085–1095.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cramer, A. O., Van Ravenzwaaij, D., Matzke, D., Steingroever, H., Wetzels, R., Grasman, R. P., et al. (2016). Hidden multiplicity in exploratory multiway ANOVA: Prevalence and remedies. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23(2), 640–647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Doricchi, F., Macci, E., Silvetti, M., & Macaluso, E. (2010). Neural correlates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in the Posner task. Cerebral Cortex, 20(7), 1574–1585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dye, M. W., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2009). The development of attention skills in action video game players. Neuropsychologia, 47(8–9), 1780–1789.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • English, M. C., Maybery, M. T., & Visser, T. A. (2015). Individuals with autistic-like traits show reduced lateralization on a greyscales task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(10), 3390–3395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • English, M. C. W., Maybery, M. T., & Visser, T. A. W. (2017). Reduced pseudoneglect for physical space, but not mental representations of space, for adults with autistic traits. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(7), 1956–1965.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, K. L., & Hampson, E. (2015). Sex-dependent effects on tasks assessing reinforcement learning and interference inhibition. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1044.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., Gu, X., Guise, K. G., Liu, X., Fossella, J., Wang, H., et al. (2009). Testing the behavioral interaction and integration of attentional networks. Brain and Cognition, 70(2), 209–220.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J. I., & Posner, M. I. (2005). The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage, 26(2), 471–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Feng, Q., Zheng, Y., Zhang, X., Song, Y., Luo, Y. J., Li, Y., et al. (2011). Gender differences in visual reflexive attention shifting: evidence from an ERP study. Brain Research, 1401, 59–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geng, J. J., & Vossel, S. (2013). Re-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: Contextual updating? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10 Pt 2), 2608–2620.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, D. J., Barnea, A., Herzberg, K., Rassis, A., Neta, M., Raz, A., et al. (2008). Measuring attention in the hemispheres: the lateralized attention network test (LANT). Brain and Cognition, 66(1), 21–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gur, R. C., & Gur, R. E. (2017). Complementarity of sex differences in brain and behavior: From laterality to multimodal neuroimaging. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 95(1–2), 189–199.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hatta, T., & Nagaya, K. (2009). Menstrual cycle phase effects on memory and stroop task performance. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38(5), 821–827.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hausmann, M. (2005). Hemispheric asymmetry in spatial attention across the menstrual cycle. Neuropsychologia, 43(11), 1559–1567.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hausmann, M. (2017). Why sex hormones matter for neuroscience: A very short review on sex, sex hormones, and functional brain asymmetries. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 95, 40–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hirnstein, M., Hugdahl, K., & Hausmann, M. (2019). Cognitive sex differences and hemispheric asymmetry: A critical review of 40 years of research. Laterality, 24(2), 204–252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 373–398.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S., Bigler, R. S., Joel, D., Tate, C. C., & van Anders, S. M. (2019). The future of sex and gender in psychology: Five challenges to the gender binary. American Psychologist, 74(2), 171–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jewell, G., & McCourt, M. E. (2000). Pseudoneglect: a review and meta-analysis of performance factors in line bisection tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 93–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joel, D., Berman, Z., Tavor, I., Wexler, N., Gaber, O., Stein, Y., et al. (2015). Sex beyond the genitalia: The human brain mosaic. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(50), 15468–15473.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M., & Ivanoff, J. (2010). The components of visual attention and the ubiquitous Simon effect. Acta Psychologica, 136(2), 225–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krall, S. C., Rottschy, C., Oberwelland, E., Bzdok, D., Fox, P. T., Eickhoff, S. B., et al. (2015). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in attention and social interaction as revealed by ALE meta-analysis. Brain Structure and Function, 220(2), 587–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Li, Y., Jin, X., Wang, Y., & Niu, D. (2019). Reduced lateralization of attention in action video game players. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1631.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, G., Hu, P. P., Fan, J., & Wang, K. (2013). Gender differences associated with orienting attentional networks in healthy subjects. Chinese Medical Journal, 126(12), 2308–2312.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marotta, A., Lupianez, J., & Casagrande, M. (2012). Investigating hemispheric lateralization of reflexive attention to gaze and arrow cues. Brain and Cognition, 80(3), 361–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marzecová, A., Asanowicz, D., Kriva, L., & Wodniecka, Z. (2013). The effects of bilingualism on efficiency and lateralization of attentional networks. Bilingualism Language and Cognition, 16(3), 608–623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, K., & Tanaka, K. (2004). Conflict and cognitive control. Science, 303(5660), 969–970.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, M. M., Woolley, C. S., & Arnold, A. P. (2017). Incorporating sex as a biological variable in neuroscience: What do we gain? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(12), 707–708.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGlone, J. (1980). Sex differences in human brain asymmetry: A critical survey. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3(2), 215–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt, P., Hirshman, E., Wharton, W., Stangl, B., Devlin, J., & Lenz, A. (2007). Evidence for gender differences in visual selective attention. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(3), 597–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2014). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 37–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 73–89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pletzer, B., Harris, T., & Ortner, T. M. (2017). Sex and menstrual cycle influences on three aspects of attention. Physiology and Behavior, 179, 384–390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1978). Mental chronometry. Chronometric explorations of mind (pp. 1–24). Hillsdale: Erlbraum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, D. (2012). Gender, culture, and sex-typed cognitive abilities. PLoS ONE, 7(7), e39904.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ruigrok, A. N., Salimi-Khorshidi, G., Lai, M. C., Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M. V., Tait, R. J., et al. (2014). A meta-analysis of sex differences in human brain structure. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 39, 34–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shaqiri, A., Roinishvili, M., Grzeczkowski, L., Chkonia, E., Pilz, K., Mohr, C., et al. (2018). Sex-related differences in vision are heterogeneous. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 7521.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R., & Wolf, J. D. (1963). Choice reaction times as a function of angular stimulus-response correspondence and age. Ergonomics, 6, 99–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Śmigasiewicz, K., Asanowicz, D., Westphal, N., & Verleger, R. (2015). Bias for the left visual field in rapid serial visual presentation: Effects of additional salient cues suggest a critical role of attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(2), 266–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spagna, A., Kim, T. H., Wu, T., & Fan, J. (2020). Right hemisphere superiority for executive control of attention. Cortex, 122, 263–276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spagna, A., Martella, D., Fuentes, L. J., Marotta, A., & Casagrande, M. (2016). Hemispheric modulations of the attentional networks. Brain and Cognition, 108, 73–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoet, G. (2010). Sex differences in the processing of flankers. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 63(4), 633–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoet, G. (2017). Sex differences in the Simon task help to interpret sex differences in selective attention. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 81(3), 571–581.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiel, C. M., & Fink, G. R. (2008). Effects of the cholinergic agonist nicotine on reorienting of visual spatial attention and top-down attentional control. Neuroscience, 152(2), 381–390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thiel, C. M., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2005). Nicotine modulates reorienting of visuospatial attention and neural activity in human parietal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30(4), 810–820.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thimm, M., Weis, S., Hausmann, M., & Sturm, W. (2014). Menstrual cycle effects on selective attention and its underlying cortical networks. Neuroscience, 258, 307–317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1978). Methods of modeling capacity in simple processing systems. In J. Castellan & F. Restle (Eds.), Cognitive theory (3rd ed., pp. 200–239). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urbanek, C., Neuhaus, A. H., Opgen-Rhein, C., Strathmann, S., Wieseke, N., Schaub, R., et al. (2009). Attention network test (ANT) reveals gender-specific alterations of executive function in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 168(2), 102–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van den Hurk, P. A., Giommi, F., Gielen, S., Speckens, A. E., & Barendregt, H. (2010). Greater efficiency in attentional processing related to mindfulness meditation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(6), 1168–1180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, J. J., Bowers, C. A., & Vogel, D. S. (2003). Cerebral lateralization of spatial abilities: A meta-analysis. Brain and Cognition, 52(2), 197–204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vossel, S., Geng, J. J., & Fink, G. R. (2014). Dorsal and ventral attention systems: Distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist, 20(2), 150–159.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vossel, S., Mathys, C., Daunizeau, J., Bauer, M., Driver, J., Friston, K. J., et al. (2014). Spatial attention, precision, and Bayesian inference: A study of saccadic response speed. Cerebral Cortex, 24(6), 1436–1450.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vossel, S., Thiel, C. M., & Fink, G. R. (2006). Cue validity modulates the neural correlates of covert endogenous orienting of attention in parietal and frontal cortex. Neuroimage, 32(3), 1257–1264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vossel, S., Thiel, C. M., & Fink, G. R. (2008). Behavioral and neural effects of nicotine on visuospatial attentional reorienting in non-smoking subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33(4), 731–738.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117(2), 250–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, K., Fan, J., Dong, Y., Wang, C., Lee, T. M., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Selective impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 78(2), 235–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weis, S., Hausmann, M., Stoffers, B., Vohn, R., Kellermann, T., & Sturm, W. (2008). Estradiol modulates functional brain organization during the menstrual cycle: An analysis of interhemispheric inhibition. The Journal of Neuroscience, 28(50), 13401–13410.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Xuan, B., Mackie, M. A., Spagna, A., Wu, T., Tian, Y., Hof, P. R., et al. (2016). The activation of interactive attentional networks. NeuroImage, 129, 308–319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zell, E., Krizan, Z., & Teeter, S. R. (2015). Evaluating gender similarities and differences using metasynthesis. American Psychologist, 70(1), 10–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank two anonymous reviewers and Shen-Mou Hsu for their constructive comments. Y.L. was supported by the Humanities and Social Sciences Fund of Ministry of Education of China (20YJCZH079). Y.L. and H.D.R. were supported by the BNU-HKBU United International College External Research Grant (R6014) and Internal Research Grants (R202011, R201710). G.W.H was supported by the BNU-HKBU United International College Research Grant (R5201814). L.Z. was supported by Social Science Fund of Beijing (17YYA004).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ghee Wee Ho.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

All the authors declare there is no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from each of the participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 455 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Li, Y., Wang, Y., Jin, X. et al. Sex differences in hemispheric lateralization of attentional networks. Psychological Research 85, 2697–2709 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01423-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01423-z

Navigation