Skip to main content
Log in

The deceptive nature of associative word pairs: the effects of associative direction on judgments of learning

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The accuracy of judgments of learning (JOLs) in forecasting later recall of cue–target pairs is sensitive to associative direction. JOLs are generally well calibrated for forward associative pairs (e.g., credit-card), but recall accuracy is often overestimated for backward pairs (e.g., card-credit). The present study further examines the effect of associative direction on JOL accuracy by comparing forward and backward pairs to unrelated pairs and symmetrical associates (e.g., salt–pepper)—a novel comparison. The correspondence between initial JOLs and recall accuracy was examined when study was either self-paced with concurrent JOLs (Experiment 1), when study/JOL duration was equated across pair types (Experiment 2), when JOLs were made immediately following study (Experiment 3), and when JOLs were made after a delay (Experiment 4). Across experiments, JOLs accurately estimated correct recall for forward pairs, but overestimated recall for symmetrical, backward, and unrelated pairs—an overestimation that was particularly robust for backward pairs. Calibration plots depicting JOL ratings against their corresponding recall accuracy indicated overestimations occurred for all pair types, though overestimations only occurred at high JOL ratings for symmetrical and forward pairs, a qualitative difference that was not captured in standard analyses of mean JOL and recall rates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Analyses were also conducted on datasets with no imputation and with the imputation done only for participants missing 5% or less of their total JOL responses. Since similar data were found using each imputation method, we report the results using the 10% cutoff criterion which maximized the number of observations available for analyses. Datasets using no imputation and the 5% cutoff criterion are available via our OSF page (https://osf.io/hvdma/).

References

  • Arbuckle, T. Y., & Cuddy, L. L. (1969). Discrimination of item strength at time of presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81(1), 126–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aust, F., & Barth, M. (2018). papaja: Create APA manuscripts with R Markdown. R Package. https://github.com/crsh/papaja. Accessed 19 Dec 2019.

  • Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchsison, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., et al. (2007). The English lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 445–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, A. B., & Castel, A. D. (2018). On belief and fluency in the construction of judgments of learning: Assessing and altering the direct effects of belief. Acta Psychologica, 186, 27–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977–990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castel, A. D., McCabe, D. P., & Roediger, H. L. (2007). Illusions of competence and overestimation of associative memory for identical items: Evidence from judgments of learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(1), 107–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connor, L. T., Dunlosky, J., & Hertzog, C. (1997). Age-related differences in absolute but not relative metamemory accuracy. Psychology & Aging, 12(1), 50–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Matvey, G. (2001). Empirical analysis of the intrinsic–extrinsic distinction of judgments of learning (JOLs): Effects of relatedness and serial position on JOLs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(5), 1180–1191.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Nelson, T. O. (1992). Importance of the kind of cue for judgments of learning (JOL) and the delayed-JOL effect. Memory & Cognition, 20(4), 374–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, L., & Kruskal, W. (1954). Measures of association for cross classifications. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 49, 732–764.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertzog, C., Dixon, R. A., Hultsch, D. F., & MacDonald, S. W. S. (2003). Latent change models of adult cognition: Are changes in processing speed and working memory associated with changes in episodic memory? Psychology and Aging, 18(4), 755–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchison, K. A. (2003). Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(4), 785–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juslin, P., Olsson, N., & Winman, A. (1996). Calibration and diagnosticity of confidence in eyewitness identification: Comments on what can be inferred from the low confidence-accuracy correlation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(5), 1304–1316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1981). Semantic facilitation in lexical decision as a function of prime-target association. Memory & Cognition, 9(6), 587–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126(4), 349–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. (2005). Illusions of competence in monitoring one’s knowledge during study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(2), 187–194. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.2.187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. (2006). Illusions of competence during study can be remedied by manipulations that enhance learners’ sensitivity to retrieval conditions at test. Memory & Cognition, 34(5), 959–972.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, W. S. (2007). Judgments of associative memory. Cognitive Psychology, 54(4), 319–353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.08.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, T. M., & Geraci, L. (2014). Improving metacognitive accuracy: How failing to retrieve practice items reduces overconfidence. Consciousness and Cognition, 29, 131–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, M. L., Dunlosky, J., & Tauber, S. K. (2015). Why is knowledge updating after task experience incomplete? Contributions of encoding experience, scaling artifact, and inferential deficit. Memory & Cognition, 43(2), 180–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Dennis, S. (2000). What is free association and what does it measure? Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 887–899. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03209337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (2004). The University of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(3), 402–407. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Dunlosky, J. (1991). When people’s judgments of learning (JOLs) are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The delayed-JOL effect. Psychological Science, 2, 267–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. Washington, DC: American Psychologist.

    Google Scholar 

  • Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [E-Prime 3.0]. (2016). https://www.pstnet.com.

  • Rhodes, G. M. (2016). Judgments of learning: Methods, data, and theory. In J. Dunlosky & S. K. Tauber (Eds.), The oxford handbook of metamemory (pp. 90–117). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G. M., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: Evidence for metacognitive illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General., 137(4), 615–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, G. M., & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The influence of delaying judgments of learning on metacognitive accuracy: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 131–148. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Wixted, J. H., & DeSoto, K. A. (2012). The curious complexity between confidence and accuracy in reports from memory. In L. Nadel & W. P. Sinnott-Armstrong (Eds.), Memory and law (pp. 84–108). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sauer, J., Brewer, N., Zweck, T., & Weber, N. (2010). The effect of retention interval on the confidence—accuracy relationship for eyewitness identification. Law and Human Behavior, 34, 337–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheck, P., Meeter, M., & Nelson, T. O. (2004). Anchoring effects in the absolute accuracy of immediate versus delayed judgments of learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 71–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tekin, E., & Roediger, H. L. (2017). The range of confidence scales does not affect the relationship between confidence accuracy in recognition memory. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2(1), 49–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tiede, H. L., & Leboe, J. P. (2009). Metamemory judgments and the benefits of repeated study: Improving recall predictions through the activation of appropriate knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 822–828.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1974). Cue-dependent forgetting. American Scientist, 62(1), 74–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valentine, K. D., & Buchanan, E. M. (2013). JAM-boree: An application of observation oriented modelling to judgements of associative memory. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 400–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Buuren, S., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2011). Mice: Mulitvariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45(3), 1–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Overschelde, J. P., & Nelson, T. O. (2006). Delayed judgments of learning cause both a decrease in absolute accuracy (calibration) and an increase in relative accuracy (resolution). Memory & Cognition, 34(7), 1527–1538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, N., & Brewer, N. (2003). The effect of judgment type and confidence scale on confidence-accuracy calibration in face recognition. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 490–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicholas P. Maxwell.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

The studies reported were approved by the University of Southern Mississippi Institutional Review Board (Protocol #IRB-19-429) and found to be in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration ethical principles. Informed consent was obtained from all individuals who participated in this study. The authors report no completing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

R code used for data screening and analyses as well as all applicable stimuli and data files have been made available on our OSF page (https://osf.io/hvdma/). All code is embedded inline within the manuscript in an R markdown document written with the papaja package (Aust & Barth, 2018).

Appendix

Appendix

See Tables 1, 2, and 3.

Table 1 Summary Statistics for associative overlap variables
Table 2 Summary statistics for cue and target item properties
Table 3 Comparison of mean JOL ratings and correct recall percentages across all associative direction groups for each experimental manipulation and pooled analysis

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Maxwell, N.P., Huff, M.J. The deceptive nature of associative word pairs: the effects of associative direction on judgments of learning. Psychological Research 85, 1757–1775 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01342-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01342-z

Navigation