Skip to main content
Log in

Multiple processing limitations underlie multitasking costs

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Human multitasking is typically defined as the practice of performing more than one task at the same time (dual task) or rapidly alternating between multiple tasks (task switching). The majority of research in multitasking has been focusing on individual paradigms, with surprisingly little effort in understanding their relationships. We adopted an individual-difference approach to reveal the limitations underlying multitasking costs measured in different paradigms. Exploratory factor analyses revealed not a general multitasking factor but instead three different processing limitations associated with response selection, retrieval and maintenance of task information, and task-set reconfiguration. The three factors were only weakly correlated with and thus not reducible to common measures of processing speed, working memory capacity and fluid intelligence. Males and females excelled in different aspects of multitasking, demonstrating the benefit of using a multifaceted view of multitasking competency in group comparison. Findings of the current study help resolve conflicting results between studies using different paradigms, and form the basis of more comprehensive measurement tools and training protocols covering different aspects of multitasking limitations. The study will also help future integration of multitasking abilities into the theoretical framework of executive function.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Eight multitasking paradigms with 14 multitasking cost indicators were initially identified. We decided to exclude two paradigms after a pilot testing with 20 participants. The continuous tracking—word generation paradigm (Strayer & Johnston, 2001)—was discarded due to its lower reliability and the high correlation between tracking errors of the tracking tasks in this paradigm and that in the continuous tracking—working memory span paradigms. An interruption paradigm (Bai, Jones, Moss, & Doane, 2014) was also included in the pilot study and excluded in the actual experiment due to the low reliability of the interruption and resumption costs.

  2. A difference between the text entry task adopted in Borst et al. (2010) and that in the current study was that Borst et al. (2010) required participants to enter the letters by clicking on an on-screen keypad while participants entered the letters by simply using the keyboard in the current study. Due to this difference, participants in the current study were able to move their fingers to the next letter on the keyboard during the subtraction task in the hard condition. As a result, they could respond very quickly to the text entry task in the next trial.

  3. Velicer’s minimum average partial test (MAP) was not performed in the current study, as the MAP method showed consistent underestimation of the number of factors when the number of variables per component was small (Zwick & Velicer, 1986).

References

  • Achtman, R. L., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2008). Video games as a tool to train visual skills. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 26, 435–446.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, H., & Nilsson, L. (1995). The effects of a mobile telephone task on driver behaviour in a car following situation. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 27, 707–715.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bai, H., Jones, W. E., Moss, J., & Doane, S. M. (2014). Relating individual differences in cognitive ability and strategy consistency to interruption recovery during multitasking. Learning and Individual Differences, 35, 22–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.07.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Band, G. P. H., & van Nes, F. T. (2006). Reconfiguration and the bottleneck: Does task switching affect the refractory-period effect? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 593–623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bherer, L., Kramer, A. F., Peterson, M. S., Colcombe, S., Erickson, K., & Becic, E. (2008). Transfer effects in task-set cost and dual-task cost after dual-task training in older and younger adults: Further evidence for cognitive plasticity in attentional control in late adulthood. Experimental Aging Research, 34, 188–219. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610730802070068.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2008). The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta Psychologica, 129, 387–398.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bors, D. A., & Stokes, T. L. (1998). Raven’s advanced progressive matrices: Norms for first-year university students and the development of a short form. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58(3), 382–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borst, J. P., Taatgen, N. A., & van Rijn, H. (2010). The problem state: a cognitive bottleneck in multitasking. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(2), 363–382.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colom, R., Martinez-Molina, A., Shih, P. C., & Santacreu, J. (2010). Intelligence, working memory, and multitasking performance. Intelligence, 38, 543–551.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dux, P. E., Tombu, M. N., Harrison, S., Rogers, B. P., Tong, F., & Marois, R. (2009). Training improves multitasking performance by increasing the speed of information processing in human prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 63, 127–138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2009.06.005.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Elsmore, T. F. (1994). SYNWORK1: A PC-based tool for assessment of performance in a simulated work environment. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 26, 412–426. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03204659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 309–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4(3), 272–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foehr, U. G., Rideout, V. J., & Roberts, D. F. (2005). Generation M: media in the lives of 8–18-year-olds. Menlo Park: Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fothergill, S., Loft, S., & Neal, A. (2009). ATC-labAdcanced: An air traffic control simulator with realism and control. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 118–127. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.1.118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17(2), 172–179.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glover, S., & Dixon, P. (2004). Likelihood ratios: A simple and flexible statistic for empirical psychologists. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(5), 791–806. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hambrick, D. Z., Oswald, F. L., Darowski, E. S., Rench, T. A., & Brou, R. (2010). Predictors of multitasking performance in a synthetic work paradigm. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 1149–1167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hembrooke, H., & Gay, G. (2003). The laptop and the lecture: The effects of multitasking in learning environments. Journal of Computing in Higher Education, 15(1), 46–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, L., Mo, L., & Li, Y. (2012). Measuring the interrelations among multiple paradigms of visual attention: An individual differences approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perceptual Performance, 38(2), 414–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift. Archives of Psychology, 14, 89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, K., Mayr, U., & Rosler, F. (2008). Is task switching nothing but cue priming? Evidence from ERPs. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(1), 74–84. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.8.1.74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konig, C. J., Buhner, M., & Murling, G. (2005). Working memory, fluid intelligence, and attention are predictors of multitasking performance, but polychronicity and extraversion are not. Human Performance, 18(3), 243–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, A. F., Larish, J. F., & Strayer, D. L. (1995). Training for attentional control in dual task settings: A comparison of young and old adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1(1), 50–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lague-Beauvais, M., Gagnon, C., Castonguay, N., & Bherer, L. (2013). Individual differences effects on the psychological refractory period. SpringerPlus, 2(1), 368–377. https://doi.org/10.1186/2193-1801-2-368.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J., Pashler, H., & Boer, E. (2006). Central interference in driving: Is there any stopping the psychological refractory period? Psychological Science, 17, 228–235.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, L. (2009). Breadth-biased versus focused cognitive control in media multitasking behaviors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(37), 15521–15522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Bundesen, C. (2003). Clever homunculus: Is there an endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 575–599.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Gordon, R. D. (2001). Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychological Review, 108, 393–434.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Schneider, D. W. (2010). Distinguishing reconfiguration and compound-cue retrieval in task switching. Psychologica Belgica, 50, 413–433. https://doi.org/10.5334/pb-50-3-4-413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lui, K. F. H., & Wong, A. C.-N. (2012). Does media multitasking always hurt? A positive correlation between multitasking and multisensory integration ability. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(4), 647–653. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0245-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lussier, M., Gagnon, C., & Bherer, L. (2012). An investigation of response and stimulus modality transfer effects after dual-task training in younger and older. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U. (2003). Towards principles of executive control: How mental sets are selected. In R. H. Kluwe, G. Lüer, & F. Rösler (Eds.), Principles of learning and memory (pp. 223–240). Berlin: Birkhäuser.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2003). Differential effects of cue changes and task changes on task-set selection costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(3), 362–372.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104, 3–65.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Ulrich, R. (2013). Mental chronometry and individual differences: Modeling reliabilities and correlations of reaction time means and effect sizes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 819–858. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0404-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 106(37), 15583–15587.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 220–244.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. (2000). Task switching and multi-task performance. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: Control of cognitive processes (pp. 277–309). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 19–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1985). A manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. London: H. K. Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redelmeier, D. A., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1997). Association between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. The New England Journal of Medicine, 336, 453–458.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 359–379. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029082.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Meier, M. E., Montroy, J. J., Hicks, K. L., Unsworth, N., … Engle, R. W. (2016). Cognitive predictors of a common multitasking ability: contributions from working memory, attention control, and fluid intelligence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(11), 1473–1492. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reissland, J., & Manzey, D. (2016). Serial or overlapping processing in multitasking as individual preference: Effects of stimulus preview on task switching and concurrent dual-task performance. Acta Psychologica, 168, 27–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.04.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, O., & Meiran, N. (2005). On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 1477–1491.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruthruff, E., Van Selst, M., Johnston, J. C., & Remington, R. W. (2006). How does practice reduce dual-task interference: Integration, automatization, or just stage-shortening? Psychological Research, 70, 125–142.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salvucci, D. D., & Taatgen, N. A. (2008). Threaded cognition: An integrated theory of concurrent multitasking. Psychological Review, 115(1), 101–130.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sana, F., Weston, T., & Cepeda, N. J. (2013). Laptop multitasking hinders classroom learning for both users and nearby peers. Computers & Education, 62, 24–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2012.10.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, F., & Voss, A. (2014). Components of task switching: A closer look at task switching and cue switching. Acta Psychologica, 151, 184–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.06.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schumacher, E. H., Lauber, E. J., Glass, J. M., Zurbriggen, E. L., Gmeindl, L., Kieras, D. E., & Meyer, D. E. (1999). Concurrent response-selection processes in dual-task performance: Evidence for adaptive executive control of task-scheduling. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 791–814.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumacher, E. H., Seymour, T., Glass, J., Fencsik, D., Lauber, E., Kieras, D., & Meyer, D. (2001). Virtually perfect time sharing in dual-task performance: Uncorking the central cognitive bottleneck. Psychological Science, 12, 101–108.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shinar, D., Tractinsky, N., & Compton, R. (2005). Effects of practice, age, and task demands, on interference from a phone task while driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37, 315–326.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skogsberg, K., Grabowecky, M., Wilt, J., Revelle, W., Iordanescu, L., & Suzuki, S. (2015). A relational structure of voluntary visual-attention abilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 761. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039000.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoet, G., O’Connor, D. B., Conner, M., & Laws, K. R. (2013). Are women better than men at multi-tasking? BMC Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-7283-1-18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological Science, 12, 462–466.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strobach, T., Frensch, P. A., Soutschek, A., & Schubert, T. (2011). Investigation on the improvement and transfer of dual-task coordination skills. Psychological Research, 76, 794–811. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0381-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tombu, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (2004). Virtually no evidence for virtually perfect time-sharing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30(5), 795–810. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.30.5.795.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trafton, J. G., Altmann, E. M., Brock, D. P., & Mintz, F. E. (2003). Preparing to resume an interrupted task: Effects of prospective goal encoding and retrospective rehearsal. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 58, 583–603. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1071-5819(03)00023-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Selst, M., Ruthruff, E., & Johnston, J. C. (1999). Can practice eliminate the Psychological refractory period effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1268–1283.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Velicer, W. F., & Jackson, D. N. (1990). Component analysis versus common factor analysis: some issues in selecting an appropriate procedure. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25(1), 1–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wickens, C. D. (2008). Multiple resources and mental workload. Human Factors, 50(3), 449–455.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, O., Herzmann, G., Kunina, O., Danthiir, V., Schacht, A., & Sommer, W. (2010). Individual differences in perceiving and recognizing faces—One element of social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 530–548.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwick, W. R., & Velicer, W. F. (1986). Comparison of five rules for determining the number of components to retain. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 432–442.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kelvin F. H. Lui.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

This research was supported by the General Research Fund (14645416) from the Research Grants Council of Hong Kong to A.W.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (XLSX 43 kb)

Supplementary material 2 (DOCX 379 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lui, K.F.H., Wong, A.CN. Multiple processing limitations underlie multitasking costs. Psychological Research 84, 1946–1964 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01196-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01196-0

Navigation