Skip to main content
Log in

Time dependency of the SNARC effect for different number formats: evidence from saccadic responses

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In line with the suggestion that the strength of the spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect was time dependent, the aim of the present study was to assess whether the association strength depends on the processing time of numerical quantity and/or of the time to initiate responses. More specifically, we examined whether and how the SNARC effect could be modulated by number format and effector type. Experiment 1 compared the effect induced by Arabic numbers and number words on the basis of saccadic responses in a parity judgment task. Indeed, previous studies have shown that Arabic numbers lead to faster processing than number words. The results replicated the SNARC effect with Arabic numbers, but not with number words. Experiment 2 was similar to Experiment 1, but this time manual responses (i.e., responses far slower than saccadic ones) were recorded. A strong SNARC effect was observed for both number formats. Further analyses revealed a correlation between mean individual response times and the strength of the SNARC effect. We proposed that the initiation times for saccadic responses may be too short for the SNARC effect to appear, in particular with the written number format for which activation of magnitude takes time. We conclude in terms of time variations resulting from processing specificities related with number format, effector type and also individual reaction and processing speed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Brysbaert, M. (2018) Numbers and language: What’s new in the past 25 years?. In A. Henik & W. Fias (Eds.), Heterogeneity of Function in Numerical Cognition. Amsterdam: Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bulf, H., Cassia, V. M., & de Hevia, M. D. (2014). Are numbers, size and brightness equally efficient in orienting visual attention? Evidence from an eye-tracking study. PLoS One, 9(6), e99499. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0099499.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bulf, H., de Hevia, M. D., & Cassia, V. M. (2016). Small on the left, large on the right: Numbers orient visual attention onto space in preverbal infants. Developmental Science, 19(3), 394–401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bull, R., Cleland, A. A., & Mitchell, T. (2013). Sex differences in the spatial representation of number. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carling, K. (2000). Resistant outlier rules and the non-Gaussian case. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 33(3), 249–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casarotti, M., Michielin, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospatial attention. Cognition, 102(1), 101–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cipora, K., & Nuerk, H. C. (2013). Is the SNARC effect related to the level of mathematics? No systematic relationship observed despite more power, more repetitions, and more direct assessment of arithmetic skill. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 66(10), 1974–1991.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damian, M. F. (2004). Asymmetries in the processing of Arabic digits and number words. Memory & Cognition, 32(1), 164–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabbri, M. (2013). Finger counting habits and spatial-numerical association in horizontal and vertical orientations. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 13(1–2), 95–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fattorini, E., Pinto, M., Rotondaro, F., & Doricchi, F. (2015). Perceiving numbers does not cause automatic shifts of spatial attention. Cortex, 73, 298–316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fias, W. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2(1), 95–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fias, W. (2001). Two routes for the processing of verbal numbers: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 65(4), 250–259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (2001). Number processing induces spatial performance biases. Neurology, 57(5), 822–826.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations. Cortex, 44(4), 386–392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 555–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., Shaki, S., & Cruise, A. (2009). It takes just one word to quash a SNARC. Experimental Psychology, 56(5), 361–366.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., Warlop, N., Hill, R. L., & Fias, W. (2004). Oculomotor bias induced by number perception. Experimental Psychology, 51(2), 91–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitousi, D., Shaki, S., & Algom, D. (2009). The role of parity, physical size, and magnitude in numerical cognition: The SNARC effect revisited.. Perception & Psychophysics, 71(1), 143–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, N., & Reynolds, M. G. (2016). Do Arabic numerals activate magnitude automatically? Evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(5), 1528–1533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: acomputational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 32.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, W. R. (1992). Picture naming. Cognition, 42(1), 61–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, M., & Fischer, M. H. (2016). Exploring the numerical mind by eye-tracking: a special issue. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 80, 325–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hesse, P. N., Fiehler, K., & Bremmer, F. (2015). SNARC Effect in Different Effectors. Perception, 45(1–2), 180–195.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, T. M. (1990). An odd effect: Lengthened reaction times for judgments about odd digits. Memory & Cognition, 18(1), 40–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keus, I. M., Jenks, K. M., & Schwarz, W. (2005). Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has its functional locus in a response selection stage. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 48–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindemann, O., Alipour, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Finger counting habits in middle eastern and western individuals: an online survey. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 566–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., & Brugger, P. (2008). Looking for the answer: The mind’s eye in number space. Neuroscience, 151(3), 725–729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher, T., Bockisch, C. J., Nicholls, M. E., & Brugger, P. (2010). Eye position predicts what number you have in mind. Current Biology, 20(6), R264–R265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loetscher, T., Schwarz, U., Schubiger, M., & Brugger, P. (2008). Head turns bias the brain’s internal random generator. Current Biology, 18(2), R60–R62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macnamara, A., Keage, H. A., & Loetscher, T. (2018). Mapping of non-numerical domains on space: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Experimental Brain Research, 236, 335–346. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5154-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mock, J., Huber, S., Klein, E., & Moeller, K. (2016). Insights into numerical cognition: Considering eye-fixations in number processing and arithmetic. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 80(3), 334–359.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Myachykov, A., Cangelosi, A., Ellis, R., & Fischer, M. H. (2015). The oculomotor resonance effect in spatial–numerical mapping. Actapsychologica, 161, 162–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myachykov, A., Ellis, R., Cangelosi, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2016). Ocular drift along the mental number line. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 80(3), 379–388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H. C., Iversen, W., & Willmes, K. (2004). Notational modulation of the SNARC and the MARC (linguistic markedness of response codes) effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 57(5), 835–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nuerk, H. C., Wood, G., & Willmes, K. (2005). The universal SNARC effect: The association between number magnitude and space is amodal. Experimental Psychology, 52(3), 187–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roettger, T. B., & Domahs, F. (2015). Grammatical number elicits SNARC and MARC effects as a function of task demands. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(6), 1231–1248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rugani, R., & de Hevia, M. D. (2017). Number-space associations without language: Evidence from preverbal human infants and non-human animal species. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(2), 352–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, W., & Keus, I. M. (2004). Moving the eyes along the mental number line: Comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(4), 651–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic, W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 328–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijck, J. P., Abrahamse, E. L., Acar, F., Ketels, B., & Fias, W. (2014). A working memory account of the interaction between numbers and spatial attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(8), 1500–1513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Viarouge, A., Hubbard, E. M., & McCandliss, B. D. (2014). The cognitive mechanisms of the SNARC effect: An individual differences approach. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e95756. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095756.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, G., Nuerk, H. C., & Willmes, K. (2006). Crossed Hands and the Snarc Effect: A failure to Replicate Dehaene, Bossini and Giraux (1993). Cortex, 42(8), 1069–1079.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: A meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science, 50(4), 489–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zebian, S. (2005). Linkages between number concepts, spatial thinking, and directionality of writing: The SNARC effect and the reverse SNARC effect in English and Arabic monoliterates, biliterates, and illiterate Arabic speakers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 5(1), 165–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We warmly thank all reviewers for considering this paper. We thank Dr Christelle Lemoine-Lardennois and Dr Alexandra Fayel for their technical skills as well as all participants who volunteered for this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alexandra Pressigout.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pressigout, A., Charvillat, A., Mersad, K. et al. Time dependency of the SNARC effect for different number formats: evidence from saccadic responses. Psychological Research 83, 1485–1495 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1010-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1010-y

Navigation