Skip to main content
Log in

Joint cognition and the role of human agency in random number choices

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Joint cognition refers to the mental systems that support group performance when carrying out a shared, or jointly owned task. We focused here on understanding the social configurations that underpin key phenomena in joint cognition, in particular, whether individual cognition in task-sharing environments is mostly shaped by social factors or not. To this end, we investigated, first and mainly, whether human presence is necessary for the creation of joint performance; second and separately, whether prior experience of task sharing has an adaptive influence on subsequent individual choices; and third and additionally, whether individual differences in a social trait mediate joint performance. We describe an experiment in which participants combined with another human or a computer as they attempted to generate a paired sequence that was as random as possible. First, we found little difference in joint performance with regard to whether a human or a computer was the co-participant, except for immediate repetitive response. Second, we found evidence for choice adaptation, but only under the lower time pressure. Third, we replicated previous research in which no systematic link was established between social desirability and joint performance. We conclude that joint cognition phenomena may be rooted primarily in turn-taking configurations rather than in social dynamics per se.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A two-way mixed ANOVA for individual immediate repetition with task order (before and after joint conditions; between-participants) and response pace (slow and fast; within-participants) as factors indicated that an interaction between the two factors was not significant, F(1, 36) = 0.07, p = 0.787, η2 = 0.002. We suggest this is because variance of immediate repetition is commonly too large. However, we suggested the necessity of closer inspection of choice adaptation and then set up a specific hypothesis that slow response pace, but not fast response pace, in individual task helped participants intentionally apply repetitive responses that they had experienced in joint task. Therefore, we administered separate analyses for the slow and fast response pace.

  2. Mann–Whitney’s U test: U = 124.0, z = 1.81, p = 0.070.

  3. Mann–Whitney’s U test: U = 143.5, z = 1.09, p = 0.278.

  4. We thank a reviewer for identifying this possibility.

  5. Mann–Whitney’s U test: U = 142.5, z = 1.08, p = 0.280.

  6. Mann–Whitney’s U test: U = 158.5, z = 0.68, p = 0.495.

  7. Mann–Whitney’s U test: U = 138.5, z = 1.24, p = 0.215.

  8. Mann–Whitney’s U test: U = 159.5, z = 0.65, p = 0.516.

References

  • Allport, A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance (Vol. XV, pp. 421–452). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1966). The capacity for generating information by randomization. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(2), 119–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brugger, P. (1997). Variables that influence the generation of random sequences: An update. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84(2), 627–661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. P. (2016). Executive functions and the generation of “random” sequential responses: A computational account. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 73(1), 153–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24(4), 349–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schutz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014a). The joint Simon effect: A review and theoretical integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 974.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014b). The joint flanker effect: Less social than previously thought. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 21(5), 1224–1230.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dudarev, V., & Hassin, R. R. (2016). Social task switching: On the automatic social engagement of executive functions. Cognition, 146, 223–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, F. J. (1978). Monitoring attention deployment by random number generation: An index to measure subjective randomness. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 12(1), 35–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2013). Social learning: An introduction to mechanism, methods, and models. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jahanshahi, M., Dirnberger, G., Fuller, R., & Frith, C. D. (2000). The role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in random number generation: A study with positron emission tomography. Neuroimage, 12(6), 713–725.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and shift (p. 89). No: Archives of Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—a review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich, G., Butterfill, S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Psychological research on joint action: Theory and data. In B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 54, pp. 59–101). Burlington: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krach, S., Hegel, F., Wrede, B., Sagerer, G., Binkofski, F., & Kircher, T. (2008). Can machines think? Interaction and perspective taking with robots investigated via fMRI. PLoS One, 3(7), e2597.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Liefooghe, B. (2016). Joint task switching. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(1), 60–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mattick, R. P., & Clarke, J. C. (1998). Development and validation of measures of social phobia scrutiny fear and social interaction anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(4), 455–470.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, B. C. N., Kühn, S., van Baaren, R. B., Dotsch, R., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2011). Perspective taking eliminates differences in co-representation of out-group members’ actions. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 423–428.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Neuringer, A. (1986). Can people behave “randomly”?: The role of feedback. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(1), 62–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2006). How the group affects the mind: A cognitive model of idea generation in groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(3), 186–213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Obhi, S. S., & Hall, P. (2011). Sense of agency and intentional binding in joint action. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 655–662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 3–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Press, C. (2011). Action observation and robotic agents: Learning and anthropomorphism. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 35, 1410–1418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 70–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2012). A 21 word solution. https://ssrn.com/abstract=2160588. Accessed 15 May 2017.

  • Stenzel, A., Chinellato, E., Tirado Bou, M. A., del Pobil, Á. P., Lappe, M., & Liepelt, R. (2012). When humanoid robots become human-like interaction partners: Co-representation of robotic actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1073–1077.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Towse, J. N. (1998). On random generation and the central executive of working memory. British Journal of Psychology, 89(1), 77–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Towse, J. N., & Mclachlan, A. (1999). An exploration of random generation among children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17(3), 363–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Towse, J. N., & Neil, D. (1998). Analyzing human random generation behavior: A review of methods used and a computer program for describing performance. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 30(4), 583–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Towse, J. N., Towse, A. S., Saito, S., Maehara, Y., & Miyake, A. (2016). Joint cognition: Thought contagion and the consequences of cooperation when sharing the task of random sequence generation. PLoS One, 11(3), e0151306.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vesper, C., Abramova, E., Bütepage, J., Ciardo, F., Crossey, B., Effenberg, A., & Wahn, B. (2017). Joint action: Mental representations, shared information and general mechanisms for coordinating with others. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 2039.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wenke, D., Atmaca, S., Holländer, A., Liepelt, R., Baess, P., & Prinz, W. (2011). What is shared in joint action? Issues of co-representation, response conflict, and agent identification. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(2), 147–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, M., Wall, H. J., & Hommel, B. (2017a). Action-effect sharing induces task-set sharing in joint task switching. Cognition, 165, 113–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yamaguchi, M., Wall, H. J., & Hommel, B. (2017b). No evidence for shared representations of task sets in joint task switching. Psychological Research, 81(6), 1166–1177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yukio Maehara.

Ethics declarations

Funding

Parts of this project were supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Global COE (Centers of Excellence) Program (D07) by Japan’s Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology. We also acknowledge the support of the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science through a Fellowship to JNT.

Conflict of interest

All authors of the present article declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Japanese Psychological Association (JPA) and the American Psychological Association (APA) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. All participants provided informed consent before participation and were fully debriefed after their sessions.

Appendix

Appendix

See Tables 4 and 5.

Table 4 Statistics of t tests or Wilcoxon’s signed rank tests for each performance measure in comparisons between individual and joint sequences (for data in Table 2)
Table 5 Statistics of t tests for the frequency of repetition lag at each lag value in comparisons between confederate, computer, and composite sequences in Fig. 2a

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Maehara, Y., Saito, S. & Towse, J.N. Joint cognition and the role of human agency in random number choices. Psychological Research 83, 574–589 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0944-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0944-9

Navigation