Skip to main content
Log in

Response-specific effects in a joint action task: social inhibition of return effects do not emerge when observed and executed actions are different

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although the inhibition of return (IOR) effect is primarily studied when people act individually, IOR is also observed in social environments where a person observes a partner’s response before executing their own response (social or sIOR). Specifically, an observer takes longer to initiate a response to a target at a location that another individual has just responded to than to another location. The present study was conducted to determine if sIOR emerges when two individuals execute different actions—one participant executed keypress responses and the other completed aiming movements to the same set of stimuli. The two conditions in the present experiment were designed to separate the effects of observing a co-actor’s target information from observing their subsequent response. In the Full Vision condition, observers saw both the target stimuli and the response of the partner. In the Partial Vision condition, observers witnessed the response of the partner, but did not see the target stimulus or any other potentially attention capturing event at the target location. It was found that, although sIOR emerged in the Full Vision condition, sIOR did not emerge in the Partial Vision condition. These and other previous findings on the impact of action goal on sIOR are discussed with reference to the potential contributions of attention and action co-representation mechanisms to the sIOR effect.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the present paper, a paired-alternating method was used in which participants execute two responses and then watch their partner execute two responses (i.e., AABBAABB…). As a consequence, individual trials (in which the person follows their own response) and joint trials (in which the person follows the action of their partner) are intermixed into the same blocks. In other studies, a straight-alternating approach has been used where participants alternate on each trial (i.e., ABABAB…). The latter straight-alternating approach may be preferred in some circumstances because it indexes the more interesting sIOR in isolation from the iIOR generated by one’s own actions. The present work aimed to assess both sIOR and iIOR and, as such, the decision was made to employ the paired-alternating approach.

References

  • Abrams, R. A., & Christ, S. E. (2003). Motion onset captures attention. Psychological Science, 14(5), 427–432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ariga, A., & Watanabe, K. (2009). What is special about the index finger?: The index finger advantage in manipulating reflexive attentional shift. Japanese Psychological Research, 51(4), 258–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atamca, S., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2011). The joint flanker effect: Sharing tasks with real and imagined co-actors. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 371–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, M. A., Simpson, A., Skarratt, P. A., & Cole, G. G. (2014). Is social inhibition of return due to action co-representation? Acta Psychologica, 150, 85–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A., & Gattis, G. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 53, 153–164.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Böckler, A., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Giving a helping hand: Effects of joint attention on mental rotation of body parts. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 531–545.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1), 3–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen through18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cisek, P. (2007). Cortical mechanisms of action selection: The affordance competition hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362(1485), 1585–1599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, G. G., Skarratt, P. A., & Billing, R.-C. (2012). Do action goals mediate social inhibition of return? Psychological Research, 76(6), 736–746.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2003). Action plans used in action observation. Nature, 424(6950), 769–771.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science, 308(5722), 662–667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 490–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frischen, A., Loach, D., & Tipper, S. P. (2009). Seeing the world through another person’s eyes: Simulating selective attention via action observation. Cognition, 111(2), 212–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, M., Reitzner, B., Corves, C., Casile, A., Giese, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Predicting point-light actions in real-time. Neuroimage, 36, T22–T32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janczyk, M., , T.N., & Dolk, T. (2016). A role of action goals for social inhibition of return? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. doi:10.1080/17470218.2015.1112417

  • Kim, D., & Hommel, B. (2015). An event-based account of conformity. Psychological Science, 26, 484–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M. (2000). Inhibition of return. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 138–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M., & Hilchey, M. D. (2011). Oculomotor inhibition of return. In S. Liversedge, I. Gilchrist, & S. Everling (Eds.), Oxford handbook on eye movements (pp. 471–492). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M., & MacInnes, W. J. (1999). Inhibition of return as a foraging facilitator in visual search. Psychological Science, 10(4), 346–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Massen, C., & Prinz, W. (2009). Movements, actions and tool-use actions: An ideomotor approach to imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 364(1528), 2349–2358.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Maylor, E. A., & Hockey, R. (1985). Inhibitory component of externally controlled covert orienting in visual space. Journal of Experimental Human Perception and Performance, 11(6), 777–787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neyedli, H., & Welsh, T. N. (2012). The processes of facilitation and inhibition in a cue-target paradigm: Insight from movement trajectory deviations. Acta Psychologica, 139, 159–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ondobaka, S., de Lange, F. P., Newman-Norlund, R. D., Wiemers, M., & Bekkering, H. (2012). Interplay between action and movement intentions during social interaction. Psychological Science, 23(1), 30–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ondobaka, S., Newman-Norlund, R. D., de Lange, F. P., & Bekkering, H. (2013). Action recognition depends on observer’s level of action control and social personality traits. PLoS One, 8(11), e81392.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Parkinson, J., Springer, A., & Prinz, W. (2011). Can you see me in the snow? Action simulation aids the detection of visually degraded human motion. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(8), 1463–1472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parkinson, J., Springer, A., & Prinz, W. (2012). Before, during and after you disappear: Aspects of timing and dynamic updating of the real-time action simulation of human motions. Psychological Research, 76(4), 421–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, J., & Neggers, B. (2008). Inhibition of return in single and dual tasks: Examining saccadic, keypress, and pointing responses. Perception and Psychophysics, 70(2), 257–265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (2005). An ideomotor approach to imitation. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science (Vol. 1, pp. 141–156). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ray, M., & Welsh, T. N. (2011). Response selection during a joint action task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43(4), 329–332.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Reviews in Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B. M. (1994). Space and selective attention. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV (pp. 231–265). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in joint action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 353–367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others' actions: just like one's own? Cognition, 88, B11–B21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skarratt, P. A., Cole, G. G., & Kingstone, A. (2010). Social inhibition of return. Acta Psychologica, 134(1), 48–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, C., Lloyd, D., McGlone, F., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Driver, J. (2000). Inhibition of return is supramodal: A demonstration between all possible pairings of vision, touch, and audition. Experimental Brain Research, 134(1), 42–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Springer, A., & Prinz, W. (2010). Action semantics modulate action prediction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(11), 2141–2158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, T. L., & Klein, R. M. (2000). Visual and motor effects in inhibition of return. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(5), 1639–1656.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tipper, S. P., Lortie, C., & Baylis, G. C. (1992). Selective reaching: Evidence for action-centred attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 891–905.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay, L., Welsh, T. N., & Elliott, D. (2005). Between-trial inhibition and facilitation in goal-directed aiming: Manual and spatial asymmetries. Experimental Brain Research, 160, 79–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Umilta, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (2001). I know what you are doing: A neurophysiological study. Neuron, 31(1), 155–165.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vesper, C., Butterfill, S., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2010). A minimal architecture for joint action. Neural Networks, 23(8), 998–1003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Villiger, M., Chandrasekharan, S., & Welsh, T. N. (2011). Activity of human motor system during action observation is modulated by object presence. Experimental Brain Research, 209(1), 85–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Elliott, D., Anson, J. G., Dhillon, V., Weeks, D. J., Lyons, J. L., & Chua, R. (2005). Does Joe influence Fred’s action? Inhibition of return across different nervous systems. Neuroscience Letters, 385(2), 99–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Lyons, J., Weeks, D. J., Anson, J. G., Chua, R., Mendoza, J. E., & Elliott, D. (2007). Within- and between-nervous system inhibition of return: Observation is as good as performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(5), 950–956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Manzone, J., & McDougall, L. M. (2014). Knowledge of response location alone is not sufficient to generate social inhibition of return. Acta Psychologica, 153, 153–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., McDougall, L. M., & Weeks, D. J. (2009a). The performance and observation of action shape future behaviour. Brain and Cognition, 71(2), 64–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., & Pratt, J. (2006). Inhibition of return in cue-target and target–target tasks. Experimental Brian Research, 174(1), 167–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Ray, M., Weeks, D. J., Dewey, D., & Elliott, D. (2009b). Does Joe influence Fred’s action? Not if Fred has autism spectrum disorder. Brain Research, 1248, 141–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., & Weeks, D. J. (2010). Visual selective attention and action. In D. Elliott & M. A. Khan (Eds.), Vision and Goal-Directed Movement: Neurobehavioral Perspectives (pp. 39–58). Champaign: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Neyedli, H. F., & Tremblay, L. (2013a). Refining the time course of facilitation and inhibition in attention and action. Neuroscience Letters, 554, 6–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, T. N., Wong, L., & Chandresekharan, S. (2013b). Factors that affect action possibility judgments: The assumed abilities of other people. Acta Psychologica, 143(2), 235–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Discovery Grant and Undergraduate Student Research Award from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and an Early Researcher Award from the Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Timothy N. Welsh.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Manzone, J., Cole, G.G., Skarratt, P.A. et al. Response-specific effects in a joint action task: social inhibition of return effects do not emerge when observed and executed actions are different. Psychological Research 81, 1059–1071 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0794-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0794-x

Keywords

Navigation