Skip to main content
Log in

Evidence for a global oculomotor program in reading

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Recent corpus studies of eye-movements in reading revealed a substantial increase in saccade amplitudes and fixation durations as the eyes move over the first words of a sentence. This start-up effect suggests a global oculomotor program, which operates on the level of an entire line, in addition to the well-established local programs operating within the visual span. The present study investigates the nature of this global program experimentally and examines whether the start-up effect is predicated on generic visual or specific linguistic characteristics and whether it is mainly reflected in saccade amplitudes, fixation durations or both measures. Eye movements were recorded while 38 participants read (a) normal sentences, (b) sequences of randomly shuffled words and (c) sequences of z-strings. The stimuli were, therefore, similar in their visual features, but varied in the amount of syntactic and lexical information. Further, the stimuli were composed of words or strings that either varied naturally in length (Nonequal condition) or were all restricted to a specific length within a sentence (Equal). The latter condition constrained the variability of saccades and served to dissociate effects of word position in line on saccade amplitudes and fixation durations. A robust start-up effect emerged in saccade amplitudes in all Nonequal stimuli, and—in an attenuated form—in Equal sentences. A start-up effect in single fixation durations was observed in Nonequal and Equal normal sentences, but not in z-strings. These findings support the notion of a global oculomotor program in reading particularly for the spatial characteristics of motor planning, which rely on visual rather than linguistic information.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A fourth gaze-contingent condition was implemented too, but failed to yield results due to a programming error in defining the gaze contingent trigger in the Experiment Builder software. The removal of this condition does not affect the other non-gaze contingent conditions and is therefore is not reported further.

  2. While segmented regressions estimate breakpoints as discrete parameters by definition, we make no inferences about whether respective transitions between regression slopes are discrete or gradual in nature (for a gradual modeling of same patterns, see mixed effects models with cubic splines in the Supplementary Materials and Fig. 1). Accordingly, discrete breakpoints in our analyses do not necessarily imply qualitative differences between processes in different segments.

References

  • Balogh, J., Zurif, E., Prather, P., Swinney, D., & Finkel, L. (1998). Gap-filling and end-of-sentence effects in real-time language processing: implications for modeling sentence comprehension in aphasia. Brain and Language, 182(61), 169–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1985). The interaction of contextual constraints and parafoveal visual information in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 17(3), 364–390.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., & Rayner, K. (1983). Parafoveal visual information and semantic contextual constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(5), 726–738. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.9.5.726.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbic, D., Glover, L., & Radach, R. (2007). The Landoldt-C string scanning task as a proxy for visuomotor processing in reading: A pilot study. In Poster session presented at the 14th European Conference on Eye Movements.

  • Ehrlich, S. F., & Rayner, K. (1981). Contextual effects on word perception and eye movements during reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20(6), 641–655. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90220-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2001). Mathematical models of eye movements in reading: A possible role for autonomous saccades. Biological Cybernetics, 85(2), 77–87. doi:10.1007/PL00008001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E. M., & Kliegl, R. (2005). SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112(4), 777–813. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Enomoto, Y., Kadono, H., Suzuki, Y., Chiba, T., & Koyama, K. (2008). Biomechanical analysis of the medalists in the 10,000 metres at the 2007 World Championships in Athletics. New Studies in Athletics, 3, 61–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, R. L., & Murray, W. S. (2000). Commas and spaces: effects of punctuation on eye movements and sentence parsing. In A. Kennedy, R. Radach, D. Heller, & J. Pynte (Eds.), Reading as a Perceptual Process (pp. 565–589). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier. doi:10.1016/B978-008043642-5/50027-9.

  • Hirotani, M., Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. (2006). Punctuation and intonation effects on clause and sentence wrap-up: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 425–443. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huestegge, L., & Bocianski, D. (2010). Effects of syntactic context on eye movements during reading. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 6, 79–87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, J. P., Marshall, R. N., & McNair, P. J. (2004). Interaction of step length and step rate during sprint running. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 36(2), 261–271. doi:10.1249/01.MSS.0000113664.15777.53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyönä, J., Lorch, R. F., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2002). Individual differences in reading to summarize expository text: Evidence from eye fixation patterns. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 44–55. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.1.44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4), 329–354.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaakinen, J. K., & Hyönä, J. (2008). Perspective-driven text comprehension. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22(3), 319–334. doi:10.1002/acp.1412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaakinen, J. K., & Hyönä, J. (2010). Task effects on eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(6), 1561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kawato, M. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9(6), 718–727. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00028-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, A., & Pynte, J. (2005). Parafoveal-on-foveal effects in normal reading. Vision Research, 45(2), 153–168. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.07.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunz, H., & Kaufmann, D. A. (1981). Biomechanical analysis of sprinting: decathletes versus champions. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 15(3), 177–181. doi:10.1136/bjsm.15.3.177.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kuperman, V., Dambacher, M., Nuthmann, A., & Kliegl, R. (2010). The effect of word position on eye-movements in sentence and paragraph reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(9), 1838–1857. doi:10.1080/17470211003602412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2015). lmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Retrieved from http://cran.r-project.org/package=lmerTest.

  • Lackner, J. R., & Dizio, P. (1994). Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory. Journal of Neurophysiology, 72(1), 299–313.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lackner, J. R., & Dizio, P. (1998). Gravitoinertial Force Background Level Affects Adaptation to Coriolis Force Perturbations of Reaching Movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 546–553.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liversedge, S. P., Drieghe, D., Li, X., Yan, G., Bai, X., & Hyönä, J. (2016). Universality in eye movements and reading: a trilingual investigation. Cognition, 147(3), 1–20. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2015.10.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, R., Kotmel, J., Herman, J., Johnson, B., & Schultz, C. (1984). Kinematic trends in elite sprinters. Proceedings of the International Symposium of Biomechanics in Sports (pp. 17–33). Del Mar, California: Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConkie, G. W., Kerr, P. W., Reddix, M. D., & Zola, D. (1988). Eye movement control during reading: I. The location of initial eye fixations on words. Vision Research, 28(10), 1107–1118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, R. K., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1990). Eye movement guidance in reading: The role of parafoveal letter and space information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(2), 268–281. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.16.2.268.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, R. E. (1984). Manipulation of stimulus onset delay in reading: Evidence for parallel programming of saccades. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 667–682. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.10.5.667.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muggeo, V. M. R. (2003). Estimating regression models with unknown break-points. Statistics in Medicine, 22(19), 3055–3071. doi:10.1002/sim.1545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muggeo, V. M. R. (2008). Segmented: An R package to fit regression models with broken-line relationships. R News, 8, 20–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pynte, J., & Kennedy, A. (2006). An influence over eye movements in reading exerted from beyond the level of the word: evidence from reading English and French. Vision Research, 46(22), 3786–3801. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.07.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.r-project.org/.

  • Radach, R., Huestegge, L., & Reilly, R. (2008). The role of global top-down factors in local eye-movement control in reading. Psychological Research, 72(6), 675–688. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0173-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1979). Eye guidance in reading: fixation locations within words. Perception, 8(1), 21–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/353867.

  • Rayner, K., Binder, K. S., Ashby, J., & Pollatsek, A. (2001). Eye movement control in reading: word predictability has little influence on initial landing positions in words. Vision Research, 41(7), 943–954.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Chace, K. H., Slattery, T. J., & Ashby, J. (2006). Eye movements as reflections of comprehension processes in reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10(3), 241–255. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr1003_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., & Duffy, S. A. (1986). Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory and Cognition, 14(3), 191–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., & Fischer, M. H. (1996). Mindless reading revisited: Eye movements during reading and scanning are different. Perception and Psychophysics, 58, 734–747.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Kambe, G., & Duffy, S. A. (2000). The effect of clause wrap-up on eye movements during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(4), 1061–1080. doi:10.1080/713755934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., & Raney, G. E. (1996). Eye movement control in reading and visual search: Effects of word frequency. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(2), 245–248. doi:10.3758/BF03212426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Morris, R. K., Schmauder, A. R., & Clifton, C. (1989). Eye movements and on-line language comprehension processes. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4(3–4), SI21–SI49. doi:10.1080/01690968908406362.

  • Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105(1), 125–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (2006). E-Z Reader: A cognitive-control, serial-attention model of eye-movement behavior during reading. Cognitive Systems Research, 7(1), 4–22. doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2005.07.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E. D., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (2003). The E-Z reader model of eye-movement control in reading: Comparisons to other models. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(4), 445–476. discussion 477–526.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reichle, E. D., Warren, T., & McConnell, K. (2009). Using E-Z Reader to model the effects of higher level language processing on eye movements during reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(1), 1–21. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.1.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schad, D. J., & Engbert, R. (2012). The zoom lens of attention: Simulating shuffled versus normal text reading using the SWIFT model. Visual Cognition, 20(4–5), 391–421. doi:10.1080/13506285.2012.670143.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Schad, D. J., Nuthmann, A., & Engbert, R. (2010). Eye movements during reading of randomly shuffled text. Vision Research, 50(23), 2600–2616. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.08.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A. (1975). A schema theory of discrete motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 82(4), 225–260. doi:10.1037/h0076770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A. (2003). Motor schema theory after 27 years: Reflections and implications for a new theory. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74(4), 366–375. doi:10.1080/02701367.2003.10609106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. A., & Lee, T. (2013). Motor Learning and performance, 5E with web study guide: From principles to application. Champaign: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shadmehr, R., & Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. (1994). Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 14(5 Pt 2), 3208–3224.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood, D. E., & Lee, T. D. (2003). Schema theory: critical review and implications for the role of cognition in a new theory of motor learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74(4), 376–382. doi:10.1080/02701367.2003.10609107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Underwood, G., Clews, S., & Everatt, J. (1990). How do readers know where to look next? Local information distributions influence eye fixations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 42(1), 39–65. doi:10.1080/14640749008401207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vitu, F., O’Regan, J. K., Inhoff, A. W., & Topolski, R. (1995). Mindless reading: Eye-movement characteristics are similar in scanning letter strings and reading texts. Perception and Psychophysics, 57(3), 352–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vitu, F., O’Regan, J. K., & Mittau, M. (1990). Optimal landing position in reading isolated words and continuous text. Perception and Psychophysics, 47(6), 583–600. doi:10.3758/BF03203111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von der Malsburg, T., Kliegl, R., & Vasishth, S. (2015). Determinants of scanpath regularity in reading. Cognitive Science, 39(7), 1675–1703. doi:10.1111/cogs.12208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von der Malsburg, T., & Vasishth, S. (2011). What is the scanpath signature of syntactic reanalysis? Journal of Memory and Language, 65(2), 109–127. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2011.02.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, T., White, S. J., & Reichle, E. D. (2009). Investigating the causes of wrap-up effects: Evidence from eye movements and E-Z Reader. Cognition, 111(1), 132–137. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.12.011.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, C. C., & Pollatsek, A. (2007). Searching for an O in an array of Cs: eye movements track moment-to-moment processing in visual search. Perception and Psychophysics, 69(3), 372–381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wotschack, C. (2009). Eye movements in reading strategies: How reading strategies modulate effects of distributed processing and oculomotor control. Universität Potsdam.

  • Wotschack, C., & Kliegl, R. (2013). Reading strategy modulates parafoveal-on-foveal effects in sentence reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 548–562. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.625094.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Noor Al-Zanoon.

Ethics declarations

Funding

This work was supported by the funding from Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council or Canada (NSERC) Discovery Grant 402395-2012, the Early Research Award from the Ontario Ministry of Research and Innovation, the National Institutes of Health NIH R01 HD 073288 (PI Julie A. Van Dyke), and the Canada Research Chair (Tier 2) award to Victor Kuperman. This work was also completed by Noor Al-Zanoon as an undergraduate thesis in the Cognitive Science of Language program at McMaster University (Hamilton, Ontario, Canada).

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Ethics

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee (McMaster Research Ethics Board, protocol 2011 165).

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 53 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Al-Zanoon, N., Dambacher, M. & Kuperman, V. Evidence for a global oculomotor program in reading. Psychological Research 81, 863–877 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0786-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0786-x

Keywords

Navigation