Skip to main content
Log in

Simon effects in change detection and change blindness

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Responses to centrally presented target stimuli are faster when they are accompanied by a task-irrelevant lateral accessory stimulus that corresponds spatially with the response hand (accessory variant of the Simon effect). In four experiments, we tested whether this effect depends on the awareness of the accessory stimulus. In a change blindness task, participants were asked to respond to a central letter that was accompanied by a lateral background change on some trials. Change blindness describes the phenomenon that even large changes may remain unnoticed when they occur simultaneously with another visual disruption, e.g., a blank screen. In a series of four experiments, a significant Simon effect was observed both when the accessory stimulus reached awareness and when it remained unnoticed. These results indicate that, based on the spatial location of an accessory stimulus, a spatial code is generated. This code interferes with the response code on the response-selection stage.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1 This criterion was not applied to Experiments 2–4 because it is possible to prepare a response to T2 before the display asking to report the change is presented. Unfortunately, the experimental code did not allow re-coding responses in Experiment 1.

References

  • Beck, D. M., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (2001). Neural correlates of change detection and change blindness. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 645–650.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1996). How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness? Trends in Neurosciences, 19, 456–459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Busch, N. A., Frund, I., & Herrmann, C. S. (2010). Electrophysiological evidence for different types of change detection and change blindness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1852–1869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coles, M. G. (1989). Modern mind-brain reading: psychophysiology, physiology, and cognition. Psychophysiology, 26, 251–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus–response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, R., Wierda, M., Mulder, B., & Mulder, L. J. (1988). Use of partial information in response processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 682–692.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., & Mazza, V. (2005). Electrophysiological correlates of change detection. Psychophysiology, 42, 328–342.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1737–1747.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., & Thornton, I. M. (2000). Change detection without awareness: do explicit reports underestimate the representation of change in the visual system? Visual Cognition, 7, 323–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galfano, G., Mazza, V., Tame, L., Umiltà, C., & Turatto, M. (2008). Change detection evokes a Simon-like effect. Acta Psychologica, 127, 186–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., Sirevaag, E. J., Eriksen, C. W., & Donchin, E. (1988). Pre- and post-stimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 331–344.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal detection theory and psychophysics. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. W. A. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial correspondence on two-choice response time. Acta Psychologica, 39, 427–439.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of reponse-code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261–268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2003). An ERP study of change detection, change blindness, and visual awareness. Psychophysiology, 40, 423–429.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap. Cognitive basis for stimulus–response compatibility: a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97, 253–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leuthold, H., & Kopp, B. (1998). Mechanism of priming by masked stimuli: inferences from event-related brain potentials. Psychological Science, 9, 263–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R. W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: a review of the Simon and the spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT-Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan, N. A. (1993). Signal detection theory as data analysis method and psychological decision model. In G. Keren & C. Lewis (Eds.), A handbook for data analysis in the behavioral sciences: Methodological issues (pp. 21–57). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, D. C. (1990). Response bias: characteristics of detection theory, threshold theory, and “nonparametric” indexes. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 401–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., & Hackley, S. A. (1992). Electrophysiological evidence for temporal overlap among contingent mental processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 195–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, A. D. (1998). Streams and consciousness: visual awareness and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 25–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitroff, S. R., Simons, D. J., & Franconeri, S. L. (2002). The siren song of implicit change detection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 798–815.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. M., Lleras, A., Grosjean, M., & Marrara, M. T. (2004). Using inattentional blindness as an operational definition of unattended: the case of a response-end effect. Visual Cognition, 11, 705–719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neumann, O., Ansorge, U., & Klotz, W. (1998). Functional differentiation in the visual cortex: basis for motor activation by not consciously perceived stimuli? Psychologische Rundschau, 49, 185–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A. (2002). Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 245–277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A. (2004). Visual sensing without seeing. Psychological Science, 15, 27–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: the need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8, 368–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (2000). On the failure to detect changes in scenes across brief interruptions. Visual Cognition, 7, 127–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schankin, A., Valle-Inclán, F., & Hackley, S. A. (2010). Compatibility between stimulated eye, target location and response location. Psychological Research, 74, 291–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schankin, A., & Wascher, E. (2007). Electrophysiological correlates of stimulus processing in change blindness. Experimental Brain Research, 183, 95–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R., & Craft, J. L. (1970). Effects of an irrelevant auditory stimulus on visual choice reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 272–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–3004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 31, 137–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stoffer, T. H., & Umiltà, C. (1997). Spatial coding with reference to the focus of attention in S-R compatibility and the Simon effect. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in S-R compatibility (pp. 181–208). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1345–1363.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treccani, B., Umiltà, C., & Tagliabue, M. (2006). Simon effect with and without awareness of the accessory stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 268–286.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tse, P. U. (2004). Mapping visual attention with change blindness: new directions for a new method. Cognitive Science, 28, 241–258.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Umiltà, C., & Nicoletti, R. (1992). An integrated model of the Simon effect. In J. Alegría, D. Holender, J. Junca de Morais, & M. Radeau (Eds.), Analytic approaches to human cognition (pp. 331–350). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valle-Inclán, F., Hackley, S. A., & De Labra, C. (2003). Stimulus–response compatibility between stimulated eye and response location: implications for attentional accounts of the Simon effect. Psychological Research, 67, 240–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valle-Inclán, F., Sohn, F., & Redondo, M. (2008). Spatial compatibility between stimulated eye and response location. Psychophysiology, 45, 279–285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vorberg, D., Mattler, U., Heinecke, A., Schmidt, T., & Schwarzbach, J. (2003). Different time courses for visual perception and action priming. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 6275–6280.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wascher, E., Schatz, U., Kuder, T., & Verleger, R. (2001). Validity and boundary conditions of automatic response activation in the Simon task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 731–751.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrea Schankin.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schankin, A., Hagemann, D. & Wascher, E. Simon effects in change detection and change blindness. Psychological Research 79, 1022–1033 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0628-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0628-7

Keywords

Navigation