Introduction

Predator and prey recognition is a key behavioural aspect influencing the life history, evolution, and distribution of animals (Peckarsky et al. 2008). A number of studies have assessed behavioural responses during predator-prey interactions, often with strong emphasis on the importance of predator recognition (Brown and Godin 1999; Policht et al. 2019). The predator recognition continuum hypothesis (Ferrari et al. 2008) states that the ability of a prey to recognise predators strongly depends on both the spatial and temporal likelihood of being predated, and on the diversity of predators occurring locally. Under this scenario, innate predator recognition abilities are favoured in environments where the probability of being attacked by predators is high, but the diversity of predators is low (Ferrari and Chivers 2009; Haddaway et al. 2014; Diquelou and Griffin 2019). On the other hand, when the diversity of predators is high and the risk of being predated is not constant (e.g. because of seasonal variation of predator occurrence), the ability of learning how to recognise predators and modulating anti-predatory responses can be more advantageous than innate recognition (Ferrari et al. 2008).

Species holding different trophic positions face strongly differing constraints (Arim et al. 2007; Kishida et al. 2009; Manenti et al. 2015). For apex predators, the main challenges are represented by intraspecific competition and the effective detection of prey. However, animals occupying intermediate levels of food webs (mesopredators) hold the dual role of being both prey and predator (Haidir et al. 2018; Roos et al. 2018). Thus, mesopredators experience both the constraints of being potential prey, and of being predators needing effective prey recognition. Thus, understanding their behavioural modulation can greatly help unravel predator-prey interaction mechanisms. Even though being a meso- or an apex predator is generally related to body size, with larger predators preying upon smaller ones (Eklov and Svanback 2006; Donadio and Buskirk 2016), the position of a species within food webs is strongly context dependent and may differ between habitats and ontogenic stage (Ritchie and Johnson 2009; Braga et al. 2019). In streams without fish, dragonfly larvae are a classical example; at early life-stages, these organisms occupy an intermediate position in the food webs while, once they grow, they become the apex predators (Hopkins et al. 2011; Start 2018).

When compared with surface habitats, subterranean environments show strong differences in the abundance and diversity of both predators and trophic resources, with consequences for food web structure and the behaviour of animals (Culver and Pipan 2019). In caves, the availability of prey is reduced (Uiblein et al. 1992; Urban 2007). In turn, lack of trophic resources limits the number of predators; thus when an epigean mesopredator species is able to colonise subterranean environments, it may easily become an apex predator (Salvidio et al. 2017; Bradley and Eason 2018). A clear example is provided by the fire salamander Salamandra salamandra, an amphibian breeding in both surface streams and underground springs (Manenti et al. 2017). In surface streams, larvae are mesopredators, being predated by both fish and dragonfly (Manenti et al. 2009), while in caves, they are apex predators (Barzaghi et al. 2017). The behavioural consequences of shifting to a higher level in the food web remain poorly known. For instance, it is not known if the shift from meso to apex predator reduces the species’ ability to recognise and avoid predator species, or increase prey detection ability through behavioural modification.

Underground environments can represent excellent natural laboratories in which to study such behavioural aspects, due to their greatly simplified trophic webs and reduced environmental fluctuations (Culver and Pipan 2019). Among European vertebrates, only one recently discovered fish (a loach of the genus Barbatula) and two amphibian caudates have stable populations in groundwater environments (Romero 2009; Behrmann-Godel et al. 2017), namely, the olm (Proteus anguinus) and the Pyrenean newt (Calotriton asper). The olm is a cave specialist that can survive and breed only in subterranean environments (Balazs et al. 2020), which it probably colonised approximately 8–20 Myr ago (Bulog 1994; Trontelj et al. 2007). Conversely, the Pyrenean newt has surface populations inhabiting headwaters, but also subterranean populations that have a certain degree of physiological and behavioural adaptation to cave life; these populations are believed to have only recently (after last glaciation) colonised subterranean environments (Hervant et al. 2000; Miaud and Guillaume 2005; Schlegel et al. 2009). Despite genetic differentiation which exists among populations from different areas of the Pyrenees, this process divergence is recent (beginning around the Last Glacial Maximum), and there is no clear distinction between surface and subterranean environments (Lucati et al. 2020). Both olms and Pyrenean newts are apex predators in their respective groundwater environments, while surface-dwelling Pyrenean newts are preyed upon by fish (Carranza and Amat 2005; Manenti et al. 2013a).

In this study, we tested two hypotheses related to the importance of predator and prey recognition in organisms shifting to a different trophic position. Predator detection decrease hypothesis: shifting to a higher level of the trophic web is expected to promote the loss of anti-predator responses to cues released by higher predators. We hypothesised that, while predator recognition is beneficial in populations inhabiting environments where higher predators are present, it may be reduced in populations exploiting safer environments, where higher predators are absent. Prey detection ability hypothesis: becoming apex predators in a resource-depleted environment should favour the enhancement of prey detection ability. We hypothesised that in subterranean environments, characterised by low prey availability (Jimenez-Valverde et al. 2017), active foraging behaviour is favoured by natural selection to increase prey detection, as observed in fire salamander larvae from cave-breeding populations (Manenti et al. 2013b). Here we performed behavioural tests to assess these hypotheses using the olm and the Pyrenean newt as model species, and measured their behavioural responses towards different chemical stimuli. An innovative method to measure stimuli detection and predator/prey recognition is to assess individuals’ lateralisation. Recent studies showed that the left body side (connected to the right cerebral hemisphere) is generally linked to fast reactions towards predators, while the right body side (connected to the left brain hemisphere) is involved in predatory behaviours (Bonati et al. 2010, 2013; Schnell et al. 2016); yet, only few studies have assessed lateralisation in urodeles (Izvekov et al. 2018). We integrated the quantification of individuals’ activity with the assessment of their lateralisation in response to different chemical cues. Movement measurement in a risky or foraging context is a classical approach to detect the effect of predators and trophic resources on behavioural patterns. A reduction in activity is an anti-predator response (Lima and Dill 1990; Bleicher 2017; Chin et al. 2018), while in the absence of stress or in presence of attractive stimuli (e.g. cues released by prey), individuals should spend more time in exploring the environment (Meager and Batty 2007; Hughes et al. 2010).Lateralised responses reflect the asymmetric processing of sensory inputs in the contralateral brain hemisphere, and are believed to improve composited motor activities such as recognising potential predators and in escaping (Fernandes et al. 2018). Detecting behavioural differences between populations can be challenging; thus, we used lateralised responses to better understand the relative role of predator and prey stimuli on our target species.

Material and methods

Animals

We performed our study using newts and olms reared in the Subterranean Laboratory of Moulis (Ariège, France). The study was performed in August 2016 and involved 39 adult newts (20 males and 19 females) and eight olms. Pyrenean newts belonging to five caves (20 individuals) and three streams (19 individuals) had been collected in 2007. The caves of origin are Cave of Labouich, Cave Bernard, Cave of Betharram, Cave of Pas du Loup and Cave of Vicdessos; the streams of origin are Ribaui, Cailla and Olhadoko. According to Lucati et al. (2020), the source populations of Pyrenean newts likely belong to two closely related lineages.

The eight olms were born in the Moulis cave laboratory between 1970 and 1976. The founder individuals of the breeding population of olms were collected in 1952 in the Piuka cave river near the town of Planina, Slovenia (Vandel and Bouillon 1954; Juberthie 1991).

Since their collection, Pyrenean newts and olms collected in caves have been reared and bred under darkness conditions, while newts from surface streams have been reared under a natural photoperiod using full spectrum daylight fluorescent lamps (True-light ®Natural Daylight 5500K/Colour Rendering 1A, Ra > 90) during daytime (12/12 h photoperiod). Pyrenean newts are reared in tanks of 80 × 30 × 20 cm, while olms are in tanks of 500 cm 100 × 100 cm. Pyrenean newts are hosted depending on their population of origin with at least two tanks per population; in each tank, holed bricks are provided as shelters. For both stream and cave newts, the rearing temperature is constantly maintained at 12 °C. All the individuals are fed with live Chironomus sp. and Artemia salina prey; feeding happens once a week and both newts and olms can prey ad libitum.

Behavioural tests

To assess the ability of surface- and cave-adapted urodeles to recognise potential predators and prey, we used related cues. The predator cue was obtained by collecting water from an aquarium of 80 × 40 × 40 cm in which two adult trout (Salmo trutta: one male and one female; size: 40 cm) were maintained during the experiment. The aquarium was fed by a minimal continuous water flow, and we kept the two trout for 24 h without eating before collecting the water for the tests. Previous tests have demonstrated that aquatic urodeles are able to detect and respond to this kind of predator cue (Manenti and Ficetola 2013; Winandy and Denoël 2013). Prey cues were obtained by placing in a cylindrical tank (diameter: 30 cm; water depth: 7 cm) 97 starved mayfly larvae (40 larvae of the genus Baetis, 50 Ecdyonurus, two Ephemera and five Caenis) for 24 h. We selected mayflies as they are abundant in surface streams inhabited by newts (Manenti et al. 2013a; Manenti and Bianchi 2014) and are different to the prey to which both urodele species were used to during their rearing period; the assemblage used reflects that of streams inhabited by the newts (Manenti et al. 2013a). The use of mayflies allowed us to avoid the effects of habituation (that could have arisen if we had used the Chironomus sp. or the Artemia salina with which newts and olms are normally fed), although this choice could potentially introduce a cue which the amphibians failed to recognise as coming from a prey species. We thus added to the test an additional cue, to control for responses toward unknown chemicals: lemon juice. Finally, we used water collected from the cave as a control. Each cue was freshly collected before every behavioural test session.

The experimental design consisted of multiple behavioural sessions in which the amphibians were individually exposed to the different cues (prey; predator; unknown; control) separately. For both newts and olms, behavioural sessions were performed after 7 days of starvation. The full test involved two replicates, so individuals were exposed twice to each different cue, for a total of eight trials. For newts, the test was performed both under light (600 lux) and under total darkness conditions; thus, each individual experienced 16 behavioural trials. Tests on olms were performed only under total darkness due to the sensitivity of this species to light and the likely stress induced. The order of cue presentation was completely randomized to minimise potential biases related to the sequence of cue exposure (Altmann 1974; Melotto et al. 2019).

Behavioural tests were performed in a 50 × 40-cm plastic arena (depth: 15 cm) filled with water from the cave. The regular weekly feeding of animals was arranged to have all individuals starving for 7 days prior the tests. Before starting the test, Pyrenean newts were let acclimatise in the arena for 7 min and olms for 30 min; as olms are more sensitive to stress, we decided to keep a longer time of acclimation (Uiblein et al. 1992). After the acclimation phase, 5 ml of the selected cue treatment was injected in the centre of the arena and then the trial started. Trials lasted for 5 min, during which the arena was video recorded using an H.264 DVR recording system connected to a Z210C IR camera. After trials, individuals were placed alone in a tank for 10 min recovering before the subsequent test. During this time, arenas were carefully washed three times with ground water to remove traces of cues from the previous test. After tests, individuals were fed as usual.

Video analysis and behavioural parameters

We analysed the videos using the software BORIS (Friard and Gamba 2016). BORIS is an event-logging software, which allows setting an ethogram coding behavioural displays of interest.

To describe activity and space use of both newts and olms, we considered three parameters describing their activity and space use. First we quantified total time spent in movement. Furthermore, we assessed the presence of a lateralised response by evaluating the time spent oriented in clockwise direction (exposing the right side of the body towards the centre of the arena) and the time spent oriented in anticlockwise direction (exposing the left side of the body towards the centre of the arena). In so doing, we considered individuals’ orientation and quantified the exposure of each body side towards the centre of the arena. This allowed us to measure the occurrence of a lateralised response. During behavioural tests, urodeles tend to spend most of their time close to the tank borders, a behaviour interpreted as seeking what they perceive as the safest area of the experimental arenas. Moreover, during our tests, we injected cues in the centre of the arena, which can be considered the most exposed and potentially risky area (Manenti et al. 2013b). In our experimental arena, when individuals were in a clockwise orientation, they exposed their right side to central area, which according to lateralisation theory suggests them to be acting as predators (and also more prone to being preyed upon themselves), and conversely, individuals in anticlockwise orientation exposed their left side indicating a predator avoidance behaviour. While analysing the videos, we considered individuals to be oriented if they were with at least half of their bodies inside the border area (2 cm from the arena margin) with < 45° of inclination towards the arena edge, and if their heads were not perpendicular to the border or along the bisector of the corner (Fig. 1a). When the head was perpendicular to the arena edge or along the bisector of the corner, we considered individuals to have no orientation (Fig. 1b). This produced three possible orientation conditions (clockwise; anticlockwise; no orientation), with the time spent in clockwise and anticlockwise orientation being two distinct not correlated parameters, and not merely the inverse of each other. A blind video analysis was performed by two operators: i.e. operators were not aware of the typology of trial they were going to analyse; this procedure allows us to limit biased interpretation (MacCoun and Perlmutter 2015).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Orientations considered (a) and not considered (b) in the assessment of lateralised responses of both Pyrenean newts and olms to the different cues tested during behavioural trials. We recorded the time of clockwise or anticlockwise orientation if the individuals were with at least half of their bodies in the border area parallel to the border with no more than roughly ~ 45° inclination; the time spent in all the other possible positions was not considered

Statistical analysis

We used the total time spent in movement, the time spent in clockwise orientation (exposing the right side deputed to prey search) and the time spent in anticlockwise orientation (exposing left side deputed to predator recognition) as dependent variables to build linear mixed models (LMMs): for both species, independent variables were the treatment (control, lemon, trout and prey), and for newts only, we also included the condition in which the test was performed (darkness vs. light) and the environment of origin (cave vs surface). Olms were tested in total darkness only and all shared the same origin. In models considering the time spent in clockwise orientation and the time spent in anticlockwise orientation as dependent variables, the initial orientation of individuals was added as a covariate. We tested all the possible pairwise interactions between independent variables; non-significant interactions were excluded from the final models. We included three random factors: the population of origin of the individuals (for newts only), the individual identity and the replicate. A preliminary test (GLM), found sex, snout-vent length and the operator that performed the video analysis to be not significant. Time spent in clockwise and anticlockwise orientation was log transformed to better fit a normal distribution. We first used control-treatment contrasts to compare the different experimental conditions against the controls (Field et al. 2015). Subsequently, we performed Tukey post hoc tests to assess differences between the different treatments for both newts and olms (Hothorn et al. 2008).We performed all the analyses in R 3.4.1 (R Development Core Team 2017) environment using the packages lmerTest (Pinheiro and Bates 2000) and multcomp (Hothorn et al. 2008).

Data availability

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Results

Behaviour of Pyrenean newts

Overall, we performed 684 behavioural tests. Pyrenean newts from surface populations moved on average (± SE) for 120.58 ± 2.17 s, while those from caves moved for 107.54 ± 2.97 s. Newts from caves moved on average for 146.6 ± 1.36 s when in darkness, while in light conditions, they moved on average for 143.72 ± 2.01 s; newts from streams moved for 148.7 ± 0.53 s when in darkness, while in light, they moved for 148.09 ± 0.80 s.

Both predator cues and light conditions significantly affected the total movement time of Pyrenean newts. Predator cues caused a significant reduction in the duration of movement, while light significantly increased movement (Table 1a; Fig. 2a, c). When predator cues occurred, newt movement was lower than control cues (Table 1a, P < 0.001), but also than prey cues (Tukey’s post hoc: z = − 6.46, P < 0.001) and unknown cues (z = − 5.99, P < 0.001). The movement of newts was unaffected by either prey cues (Table 1a; t577.7 = − 0.7, P = 0.48), or by the unknown cues (t578.4 = − 0.25, P = 0.79). Analyses did not show any effect of the environment of origin (t5.8 = 0.86, P = 0.42) or any significant interaction between treatment and origin (all P > 0.05); thus, both cave and surface Pyrenean newts responded similarly to predator cues.

Table 1 Behavioural responses of Pyrenean newt C. asper to the treatments and test conditions depending on their origin
Fig. 2
figure 2

Total time spent in movement by Pyrenean newts depending on the different cues used during tests (a), on the origin of the individuals (b) and on the light conditions at which we performed the tests (c). Grey area identifies 95% confidence intervals; blue line indicates median

Predator cues also affected the time spent by the newts in clockwise orientation. Predator cues caused a significant reduction of the time spent by newts in clockwise orientation (less exposure of the right side, linked to prey search; Fig. 3a); the reduction was even stronger under light conditions (Table 1b; Fig. 3c). The orientation of the animals at the beginning of the test affected the time spent in clockwise orientation (Table 1b). Moreover, with predator cues, newts spent significantly less time in clockwise orientation than with control conditions (Table 1b, P < 0.01), prey (z = − 3.75, P < 0.01) and unknown cues (z = − 4.17, P < 0.01). Clockwise orientation was unaffected by prey cues or newts’ origin. None of the factors affected the time spent in anticlockwise orientation, except for the effect of the orientation of the animals at the beginning of the experiment (Table 1c).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Time spent moving in clockwise orientation (thus exposing the right body side involved in prey detection) by Pyrenean newts depending on the different cues used during tests (a), on the origin of the individuals (b) and on the light conditions at which we performed the tests (c). Grey area identifies 95% confidence intervals; blue line indicates median

Behaviour of olms

Olms moved on average for 68.1 ± 2.94 s. Under control conditions, olms moved on average for 74.15 ± 5.23 s; with unknown scent, they moved for 77.59 ± 6.57 s, with prey cues 68.31 ± 6.30 s and with predator cues 55.11 ± 7.45 s (Fig. 4a). However, none of the treatments affected the total movement of olms, compared to the controls (all P > 0.05; Table 2). Compared with unknown cues, olms moved less when predator cues occurred (Tukey’s post hoc: z = − 3.08, P = 0.01). All the other comparisons among treatments were not significant (Supplementary Materials 1). For both orientations (clockwise and anticlockwise), we detected a significant effect of the initial orientation at the beginning of the test (Table 2). Considering the time spent in clockwise orientation (such as acting as a predator), prey cues differed to predator cues as olms showed a weak but significant tendency to spend more time in clockwise orientation with prey cues (z = 1.97, P = 0.05). We only detected a relationship close to significance when considering anticlockwise orientation: olms spent apparently less time in anticlockwise orientation when unknown cues occurred (Table 1; Fig. 4b).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Total time spent by olms moving (a) and in anticlockwise orientation (thus exposing the left body side involved in predator recognition) (b) depending on the different cues used during tests. Grey area identifies 95% confidence intervals; blue line indicates median

Table 2 Behavioural responses of Proteus anguinus to the treatments

Discussion

Our study revealed complex and heterogeneous responses of cave-dwelling urodeles to predator and prey cues. We hypothesised that shifting to a higher level in the trophic web might promote the loss of anti-predator responses (Predator detection decrease hypothesis). The limited responses in olms, which are the apex predators in groundwater, are in agreement with this hypothesis. However, this hypothesis is not confirmed for newts, as populations living in surface habitats (where fish are top predators) and populations occupying caves (safe environments) showed no difference in predator avoidance, and both maintained the capacity to express anti-predator responses when exposed to fish odours. This pattern can be explained by different processes. First of all, the isolation between surface and cave populations of the Pyrenean newt is likely very recent (Miaud and Guillaume 2005), even though a certain degree of physiological and behavioural differentiation between them already exists (Dreiss et al. 2009; Schlegel et al. 2009). Gene flow between cave and surface populations may have occurred until relatively recent times, allowing the maintenance of some behavioural traits typical of surface populations (Bachmann et al. 2020) even if populations from caves show lower genetic diversity compared with streams (Valbuena-Urena et al. 2018). Moreover, trout are likely still present in surface streams and newts could associate trout cues with the surface with its riskier environment. The retention of ability to identify physical or chemical cues typical of surface areas has been reported for some invertebrates inhabiting groundwaters that use these cues to avoid risky or suboptimal habitats. For instance, photophobic responses to light has been observed in cave amphipod crustaceans (Fišer et al. 2016) and avoidance of interface habitats rich in predators is reported for Niphargus crustaceans and planarians (Manenti and Pezzoli 2019); it is possible that the perception of predators’ chemical cues by the cave newts acts in a similar way.

Conversely, olms likely colonised caves through multiple and spatially different events that are supposed to have occurred from 8 to 20 Myr ago (Trontelj et al. 2007). All the olm lineages show strong adaptations to subterranean life, including reduction of eyes and depigmentation. Even though there are occasional reports of individuals outside groundwaters and some populations live in contact to springs and show eyed and pigmented morphotypes (Ivanovic et al. 2013), no stable surface populations exist, and isolation between populations living in different drainage catchments is strong (Voros et al. 2019). The only responses we detected was a reduction in total movement between predator and lemon cues, and a weak increase of the time in clockwise orientation with prey cues, compared with predator cues, in agreement with lateralisation patterns already described in other urodeles. Our results suggest that during the process of adaptation to subterranean environments, where they play the role of apex predators, olms decreased the ability to recognise fish as predators. Maintaining unnecessary behaviours is costly for species, so when olms adapted to a predator-free environment, the ability to detect predators and the related behavioural traits were lost through evolutionary processes (Burns et al. 2011).

On the other hand, we found no support for the prey detection ability increase hypothesis. Both Pyrenean newts and olms spent more time in clockwise orientation (thus acting as predators) with prey cues than with trout cues, and for olms, this relationship was close to the significance threshold. The prey used, from common Ephemeroptera genera, is typical of the surface freshwaters inhabited by C. asper (Manenti et al. 2013a), but rare in caves, into which they would only enter accidentally (Martynov and Vargovitsh 2015). We had chosen to use the cues of these animals instead of those of the prey with which the animals are reared in order to test for innate response. The results obtained clearly show that anti-predator responses, at least in Pyrenean newts, are stronger that those related to potential prey recognition.

Considering olms, the methods followed are similar to those applied in other behavioural studies as was the number of individuals that were available for the experiment. Considering predatory behaviour, the olm is able to detect their prey’s chemical cues over a longer distance than Pyrenean newt (Uiblein et al. 1992); olms generally explore the substrate actively searching for prey especially in the sediment (Schlegel et al. 2009). Both mechano-, chemo- and eventually electro-perception are used to detect prey (Schlegel 1997).

The only weak and not significant effect that we recorded was a tendency to expose less the left side deputed to predator detection when unknown cues occurred; moreover, olm did not show significant neophobic responses to these cues. In several subterranean environments, there is an important input of allochthonous organic matter originating from the surface (Manenti et al. 2013c; Lunghi et al. 2017), part of which can be unknown for cave species that can be attracted by curiosity/exploration. Absence of neophobic responses towards unknown cues has been observed also as a side effect in plastic and quick-breeding fish reared in safe conditions (Brown et al. 2013); however, the lack of predation risk is the natural condition of olms and their rearing conditions respect those of natural groundwaters. In our study, we could consider a relatively low number of olms, due to logistical limitations. We used a number of individuals comparable with previous evolutionary zoological studies performed on both Pyrenean newts and olms (Uiblein et al. 1992; Guillaume 2000; Hervant et al. 2001), and we performed an extensive number of tests to achieve reliable results. Even if we cannot exclude that increasing the number of olms or considering individuals deriving from different populations could evidence potential responses, we believe that the results obtained provide useful insights to extend such kind of behavioural experiment to other cave-dwelling taxa with more abundant populations. While cave-dwelling amphibians have a long history of zoological studies, but are often numerically rare, cave-dwelling invertebrates are much less studied and known, but show larger subterranean populations (Manenti et al. 2020) and can provide a tractable system for further researches.

Our results also provide insights on the importance of considering lateralisation in the behaviour of urodeles (Izvekov et al. 2018). A common basic pattern of lateralisation is likely to occur in all the vertebrates (Robins 2006). The investigation of lateralisation is important for behavioural studies on many taxa, including humans (Rogers 2010), and can allow us to increase our ability in detecting possible responses and patterns that may go unnoticed with classical parametrisations. Integrating lateralisation in the behavioural parameters allowed us to better understand the effect of predator cues. However, our results underline also that caution must be used in the assessment of lateralisation during experimental approaches. In all analyses, the initial orientation of the individuals played a significant effect on the total time spent moving in a certain orientation. Especially for less reactive and more slow-moving animals like olms (Hervant et al. 2000; Balazs et al. 2015), this could represent an aspect to be attentively considered during experiments.

Finally, newts moved more in light conditions than in darkness conditions. This result is in contrast with a similar experiment performed on salamander larvae, which have higher activity and improved foraging performance in darkness (Manenti and Ficetola 2013). It may also appear to contrast with the findings of field surveys who found Pyrenean newts to be more active at night (Manenti et al. 2013a; Miró and Ventura 2020). Pyrenean newts mainly use visual cues to catch prey (Uiblein et al. 1992, 1995); we can interpret their increased activity in light condition as a preference to actively hunt using visual cues when they have this possibility. Furthermore, the absolute darkness of laboratory conditions is very different to the situation in a natural setting where there will be moonlight and other sources of illumination, albeit at quite low levels.

For many years, organisms that are strongly adapted to subterranean environments have been key models for understanding evolutionary mechanisms (Romero 2011; Culver and Pipan 2014; Manenti et al. 2018). Taxa that are usually epigean but show some periods of subterranean activity, or have some populations colonising subterranean habitats, can be particularly important to understand the complex processes occurring during adaptation to these extreme environments (Manenti et al. 2011; Lunghi et al. 2017). The broad implication of our results is that predator recognition is maintained across populations of the same species occupying both meso- and apex predator levels, but this occurs when differentiation is recent, while the ability of detecting surface predators is lost in species with longer history of adaptation to subterranean environments.