Introduction

Dispersal has profound consequences for gene flow, the evolution of social group patterns, and the geographic distributions of animals (Clobert et al. 2012). Dispersal is usually male-biased in mammals; however, female dispersal also occurs regularly (Strier 1994) and is expected to occur when the benefits of moving to outweigh the costs (Clobert et al. 2012). In social species, the potential benefits of female dispersal include inbreeding avoidance, reduction of feeding competition, reduction of predation and infanticide risk, reduction of mate competition, mate choice, disease avoidance, and increase genetic diversity (Amos et al. 2001; Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2011) (Table 1). The potential costs of female dispersal include aggression towards immigrants (Isbell and Van Vuren 1996), loss of habitat familiarity (Bonte et al. 2012), loss of opportunity to help and be helped by kin (Young et al. 2005), or reproductive costs such as longer intervals between births (Debeffe et al. 2015; Walker et al. 2018; Manguette et al. 2019). To determine if and when to transfer, a female may gather information on other social groups and evaluate the costs and benefits of such a decision (Isbell and Van Vuren 1996; Clobert et al. 2009).

Table 1 Summary of hypotheses and variables used

Females may disperse from their natal groups to avoid inbreeding, especially if their father may still be in the group (Ims and Hjermann 2001; Clutton-Brock and Lukas 2012). If females are not able to recognize kin, a fixed strategy (unconditional dispersal) allowing females to transfer before the first parturition regardless of whether a male relative is present, may evolve to avoid inbreeding (Bowler and Benton 2005). Following natal dispersal, any subsequent “secondary” dispersal is often attributed to socioecological factors (Isbell and Van Vuren 1996; Clutton-Brock and Lukas 2012). Female secondary dispersal refers to a transfer between breeding groups and is known to occur in very few species (tropical bats: Nagy et al. 2007, feral horses: Debeffe et al. 2015, Thomas langurs: Sterck and Korstjens 2000, and gorillas: Robbins and Robbins 2015). Females may leave larger groups and join smaller groups to avoid feeding competition and intrasexual competition within groups, or reduced attraction by outsider males or predators (Hill and Lee 1998; Clutton-Brock and Lukas 2012). Conversely, larger groups may provide better protection against predators due to increased detection rates, and they may have an advantage in between-group feeding competition, especially in species with clumped-food resources (van Schaik 1989). In polygynous species, females may transfer to a male that provides better protection against infanticide or predation. If young solitary males are not yet strong enough to provide such protection, then females may be more likely to join an established group (Clutton-Brock and McAuliffe 2009). Length of male dominance tenure is expected to reflect variation in male strength and male-male contest competition, with males predicted to be more vulnerable to competitors when they have passed their prime (Steenbeek 2000; Alberts 2012). Male competitive ability is therefore expected to decline as their tenure duration increases, so it could be advantageous for females to join males early in their tenures (Steenbeek 2000; Sterck et al. 2005; Alberts 2012).

Western gorillas are an interesting species to study female dispersal because both natal and secondary transfers are common (Robbins and Robbins 2015, 2018). Female western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) live almost exclusively in one-male groups consisting of only one silverback (fully mature adult male), adult females, and immature individuals (Parnell 2002; Gatti et al. 2004; Robbins and Robbins 2018). Breeding groups begin when females transfer to a bachelor group or a lone silverback (Robbins and Robbins 2018). The silverback’s tenure ends when all females leave (and the group becomes either non-reproductive or the male becomes solitary) or when he dies and the group consequently disintegrates. The death of the silverback leads to “involuntary” female transfers to join an extra-group male, who is likely to kill their unweaned offspring (Watts 1989; Robbins and Robbins 2018). Transfers are considered “voluntary” when females switch groups during intergroup encounters while the current silverback is alive (Stokes et al. 2003). Voluntary dispersal generally does not occur when females have dependent offspring because these would be vulnerable to infanticide (Robbins and Robbins 2015). Therefore, during each 5-year interbirth interval, females have only a few months to transfer between weaning an infant and conceiving again (Sterck et al. 2005; Robbins and Robbins 2015; Sicotte et al. 2017).

Inbreeding avoidance has been considered a probable cause of natal dispersal in western gorillas, but due to group disintegrations, not all nulliparous females reach maturity in a group with their putative father (i.e., the same silverback who was dominant when the female was born). In previous studies, it was not yet clear whether natal dispersal is more likely when female western gorillas are still with their putative father or other close male relatives. Secondary dispersal by female western gorillas has typically been attributed to a potential reduction of feeding competition, disease avoidance, or increased protection against predation (e.g., leopards) and infanticidal males due to better male competitive abilities in the new group (Harcourt and Stewart 2007; Baudouin et al. 2019).

Studies of feeding competition have shown little or no influence of group size upon female fertility (Robbins et al. 2007; Manguette et al. 2019). Yet, if females leave groups as soon as feeding competition begins to intensify, dispersal may be a more sensitive measure of within-group competition than studies of female fertility (Watts 1990; Robbins et al. 2007). Infant survival was higher in larger western gorilla groups, which may arise from increased detection of predators, or because females aggregate around males who provide better protection (Breuer et al. 2010). Females may therefore prefer a group of intermediate size to account for the potentially conflicting influences of within-group competition versus protection. Stokes et al. (2003) found that female western lowland gorilla transferred preferentially to smaller groups, but Harcourt and Stewart (2007) suggested that the distribution of those transfers would be expected if females transferred randomly with respect to group size. Therefore, the potential influence of group size upon female dispersal remains to be determined.

In western gorillas, infant mortality was significantly higher during the last 5 years of the male tenure than earlier in the tenure, suggesting that those males became less capable of providing protection against predators or infanticidal males (Manguette et al. 2019). At Mbeli, the infanticide rate after group disintegration is around 12% (Breuer et al. 2010; Robbins et al. 2013). Given the high costs of infanticide after the silverback’s death, females should leave a silverback if they perceive he is weak and may not survive long enough for them to wean a new infant (Manguette et al. 2019). Females may also use group composition to assess the competitive ability of the male using criteria described in Parnell 2002 (e.g., younger groups may have fewer juveniles and subadults). Using such criteria, Baudouin et al. 2019 found that females appear to leave older groups for younger groups, but changes in female transfer patterns throughout the male dominance tenure have not been directly examined (Robbins and Robbins 2015).

To investigate the factors influencing dispersal decisions, we examined natal and secondary female dispersal patterns in a healthy population of western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) from over two decades (1995–2015) of continuous observations at Mbeli Bai in northern Republic of Congo. First, we quantify the occurrence of dispersal by nulliparous females when they were or were not with their putative father, cases of involuntary transfer, and voluntary secondary transfer. We then tested the effects of socio-ecological factors on secondary transfer probability (using only voluntary transfers as they reflect female choice) using three complementary variables: emigration rates, immigration rates, and differences between the group of origin and the group of destination (Table 1). We first tested whether dispersing females preferred smaller groups (to avoid within-group feeding competition or reduced attraction by extra-group males or predators) or larger groups (to avoid between-group feeding competition or reduce the risks of predation due to increased detection). We then examined whether females were more likely to join a silverback early in his tenure (males that are younger and have potentially greater competitive abilities) and leave a male late in his tenure and/or close to his death (when the risk of infant mortality increases either due to infanticide or predation when males have reduced competitive abilities). Because males may not be ready to start groups when they are young and inexperienced, we also tested whether females were more likely to join a solitary male or an established group. As a last point, we tested whether females dispersed more frequently after the death of an offspring (possibly indicating that the male is losing competitive ability). We discuss how these results contribute to the understanding of male and female reproductive strategies and the evolution of social systems.

Methods

Gorilla population

Western gorillas were studied at the 12.9-ha swampy forest clearing of Mbeli Bai in the Nouabalé-Ndoki National Park, Republic of Congo (Parnell 2002; Breuer et al. 2009). During the study period, no illegal human activities or major disease outbreaks were reported. Demographic data were collected from February 1995 until October 2015 on an almost daily basis, totaling 55,230 observation hours on 6140 observation days. The gorillas were observed from a 9-m-high observation platform overlooking the bai with close to 100% visibility and were identified using a combination of facial features such as body size, hair coloration, shape of the brow-ridge, and ear and nose prints (Parnell 2002; Breuer et al. 2009, 2010). For the age-sex classification of gorillas, we used categories based on Breuer et al. (2009): infants were considered dependent until weaning (4 years of age), and females were considered adult at 10 years of age and males at 18 years. Blinded methods could not be used in this study because it involved focal animals in the field.

Demographic dataset

Over the 20 years of the study, we recorded 440 gorillas including 147 females that reached weaning age (min 4 years of age) in 40 breeding groups. We observed 25 cases of natal dispersal and 285 secondary dispersal events in 1270 female-years (see Table 2). Individual gorilla groups did not visit the clearing every day, and dates of birth, presumed death, and dispersal dates were estimated. We assumed that individual gorillas were still resident in the same group if they were observed in the same group before and after the gap of observation. Because of the nature of the observation, it is possible that infants that died shortly after birth were not observed. Dispersal dates were determined as the midpoint between the last visit of the group of origin and the first visit of the group of destination (Stokes et al. 2003; Breuer et al. 2010). When a female gorilla disappeared, we were usually unable to determine whether she had died or dispersed to an unknown group outside of the study population. If silverbacks disappeared, however, we assumed that they had died as in most cases the silverbacks were seriously injured or very thin before disappearance (Breuer et al. 2010). In the case of breeding groups, silverback death resulted in group disintegration.

Table 2 Summary of dispersal and conceiving events. The same female may have more than one secondary transfer or conceiving events, so the number of females concerned in each category is in parentheses

Natal dispersal

Voluntary natal dispersal rates were calculated by dividing the number of voluntary natal dispersal cases by the total number of natal dispersal events. Involuntary natal dispersal rates were calculated by dividing the number of involuntary natal dispersal cases (when females in their natal group dispersed following the death of their putative father) by the total number of natal dispersal events. We used t tests to determine whether the type of natal dispersal (voluntary vs involuntary) had an effect on the female’s choice of group (group size). Similar tests for the variables estimating tenure duration were not possible due to sample size.

Variables for the potential influences upon secondary dispersal

To examine if feeding competition and/or predation avoidance influenced female secondary dispersal patterns, we used the size of the group (number of weaned individuals) at the time of transfer. The number of weaned individuals in a group was highly correlated with the number of adult females in the group (R = 0.78, N = 300, p < 0.001), and we therefore only used the weaned individual group size. We used a quadratic term to test for the possibility of a non-linear relationship of group size due to a combination of within- and between-group feeding competition and/or predation pressure (Markham et al. 2015; Grueter et al. 2018).

Our measures of male competitive ability were based on the tenure duration, which we calculated as the interval between the first and last dates that a silverback was observed in association with adult females. The end of a tenure was when the silverback disappeared or when all females left him. We observed only five full tenures so our analyses were based on two partial perspectives: the elapsed tenure (the duration since the tenure began) and the remaining tenure (the time until the tenure ended) (Fig. 1). Two silverbacks had an intermediate period in their tenure without any adult females and the groups were considered non-breeding groups (51 months and 13 months), and those periods were subtracted from the tenure duration. We considered male tenure duration as a better proxy than male age for competitive ability because it is likely to vary among males of the same age and because the birthdates of the males were typically unknown. Furthermore, the estimated age of the silverback was significantly correlated with the tenure duration (elapsed tenure: R = 0.92, N = 214, p < 0.001, remaining tenure: R = − 0.39, N = 127, p < 0.001). As an additional measure for male competitive ability, we used the time until the silverback died (see supplementary material S3). Lastly, we tested whether females were more likely to leave after the death of an offspring. Females may use the death of their offspring to assess the competitive ability of the male and decide whether to leave him.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Graphical description of tenure duration. Elapsed tenure is the duration since the first female joined the silverback. The remaining tenure is the time until the silverback dies or when his last female transfers out of the group (from Manguette et al. 2019, reproduced with permission of the lead author)

Data analysis

We used three different variables to test factors influencing secondary dispersal: emigration rate (when a female left the group), immigration rate (when a female appeared in the group), groups of origin and destination (when the group that the female left and the group that she joined were both known).

Emigration rate

The “emigration rate” is defined as the number of transfers divided by the number of decisions (transfers plus births). We ran generalized linear mixed models (GLMM, Baayen 2008) with a binomial error structure and logit link function to evaluate the potential influences upon secondary female transfers. The dependent variable equaled “1” whenever a female made a voluntary secondary transfer and it equaled “0” whenever she gave birth (Robbins et al. 2009c). Thus, we compared the probability of transferring versus staying in the current group to reproduce. We used two datasets; in the first, we included all secondary transfers including cases where females disappeared and in the second, we limited the analyses to transfers between known groups. The results of the second set of models are presented in supplementary material S2. Due to different datasets available for each variable and to avoid collinearity, we tested each of the potential influences as a predictor variable (group size, elapsed tenure, remaining tenure, and survival of the previous offspring to age four) in a separate model (Table 3). We controlled for the age of the female as well as a quadratic effect of female age in all models (e.g., younger females may transfer more frequently than middle-aged females due to longer time window before the first conception, and older females may transfer more frequently due to lower reproductive success, see Fig. S1). We included the identity of the female and the identity of the group as random effects as well as all associated random slopes because the individual females may transfer multiple times and females may transfer from the same group. We compared the full models with respective null models lacking the predictor variables but comprising the same random effects structure as the full model (Forstmeier and Schielzeth 2011) using a likelihood ratio test (Dobson and Barnett 2008). We assessed model stability by comparing the estimates obtained from a model based on all data with those obtained from models with each level of the random effects excluded one at a time. To rule out collinearity, we determined the variance inflation factor (Field 2005) for a standard linear model excluding the random effects. All models were implemented in version 3.5.1 of R (R Core Team 2017) using the function “glmer” of the package lme4.

Table 3 Emigration rates. Summary of mixed models used for the analyses of female voluntary transfer and comparison with their respective null model. The models used a full dataset in which we included the known transfers and the individuals that disappeared. p values considered significant (p < 0.05) are in bold and trends (p < 0.07) are in italics

Immigration rate

Group size and tenure duration

We ran a linear regression to examine the relationship between group size and immigration rates. The model contained one data point for each group size that was observed (starting at size two, excluding solitary males). The response variable was the immigration rate, which was calculated as the number of immigrations divided by the number of years that each group size was observed. The predictor variable was group size. To avoid excessive influence from group sizes with scant data, each data point was weighted according to the number of years that the group size was observed. We ran similar linear regression models to examine immigration rates versus elapsed tenure and remaining tenure.

Social unit type

We performed rate-based χ2 calculations to compare the rates of female immigration into groups versus solitary males. We tallied the number of immigrations into groups versus solitary males, and the number of years that each type of social unit was observed (unit-years). We calculated the expected number of immigrations into each type of social unit, based on the null hypothesis that the observed immigrations would be distributed in proportion to the number of years that each type of social unit was observed. We used a χ2 test to compare the expected versus actual number of immigrations.

Difference between group of origin and destination

In a subset of the secondary transfer cases, both the group of origin and the destination group were known. With this more precise dataset, we tested whether females transfer to smaller/larger groups and to males earlier/later in their tenure. We used random intercept mixed models and each model examined one of the variables for potential influences upon secondary dispersal. For each variable (group size, elapsed tenure, and remaining tenure), the response was the difference between the group of origin and the group of destination. As an example, if a female leaves a group with 10 individuals for a group with 6 individuals, the difference between origin and destination will be − 4. This method allowed us to include the random effect of the identity of the female, identity of the group of origin, and identity of the group of destination. We then bootstrapped the results to get confidence intervals. We considered that the effect was “significant” when the 97.5% confidence limits did not overlap with zero.

Results

Natal dispersal

All 25 natal females observed since birth transferred from their natal groups, 56% transferred voluntarily, 29% transferred involuntarily, and in 24% of the natal transfers, the females were not observed again so they either died or dispersed to an unknown group (Table 2). In all fourteen known cases of voluntary natal transfer, the female was in a group with her putative father, which is consistent with the hypothesis that natal dispersal is driven by inbreeding avoidance. The average age (± standard deviation) of natal females during those voluntary transfers was 9.1 ± 1.0 years (range = 7.8–10.5, N = 14). Of these fourteen females, six were subsequently observed with an infant of which 50% reproduced in the first group they transferred to while 50% transferred again, seven females were too young to breed, and one was transferred again to an unknown group or died. Natal transfers occur around 3 years prior to their first birth.

The average age of natal females making involuntary transfers was 4.8 ± 0.8 years (range = 4.0–6.0, N = 5). Of these five, two transferred together with their mother (at ages 4.8 and 5) and three transferred alone (at ages 4, 4.4, and 6). None of the females gave birth in their new group, and all of them had at least one voluntary transfer before reaching age 12. Natal females transferring involuntarily were more likely to transfer to a group with fewer females (t = − 2.754, df = 11.539, p = 0.018, mean involuntary = 1.4, mean voluntary = 4.4), but not to a smaller group (t = − 1.35, df = 9.25, p = 0.209) than females transferring voluntarily. These secondary transfers illustrate that voluntary dispersal by nulliparous females may be unconditional and females may avoid breeding with a male they knew as adult during maturation.

Secondary dispersal

Females transferred on average one time every 4.5 years (0.22 transfers per female per year: 285 secondary transfers in 1270 female-years), although females have been recorded as transferring up to twelve times and breeding in up to four different groups. There is extreme variation in the time females spend in a particular group, ranging from a few weeks and up to 20 years for females who never transferred during the study period. The number of transfers significantly increases with time a female was observed in the population (Pearson’s r = 0.396, t = 5.191, df = 145, p < 0.001) (Fig. 2). Secondary involuntary transfers after group disintegration account for 17% (N = 27, during 11 group disintegrations) of all secondary transfers (N = 163). Of the 27 females that experienced group disintegrations, 15 (56%) had unweaned infants of which 11 (73%) disappeared (age range of infants 0.1–3.5 years), confirmed by the absence of the infants in the groups the females joined. We can assume that these infants died, most likely due to infanticide (Breuer et al. 2010; Robbins et al. 2013; Manguette et al. 2019). The rare cases of unweaned offspring that survived a disintegration event (N = 4) were either offspring that were close to weaning age or that transferred with their mother to an old solitary silverback or to a non-breeding group. In such groups, often containing only immature males with or without an older silverback, infanticide is not likely.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Female time spend in the population against a number of transfers (N = 147). Each dot represents a number of transfers by one or more females (depending on the dot size) that occurred during the 1-year time period. The straight line represents the linear regression of the number of transfers against time in the population. The gray area represents the 95% confidence interval

Emigration and immigration rates

We observed 101 cases of voluntary secondary dispersal, 62 cases in which the female disappeared, and 202 cases in which the female stayed and conceived in her current group (Table 2). The emigration rate is 33% if we assume that all of the disappearances were deaths (101/303), and 45% if we assume that they were dispersal (163/365). The actual transfer probability is likely in between the two proposed values. A total of 178 immigrations by females occurred during the 566 unit-years observed (groups and solitary males), resulting in an average rate of one immigration into a group or to a solitary male every 3 years (0.31 immigrations per unit-year).

Effect of social unit type

The immigration rate to solitary males was on average one immigration every 6 years to a solitary male (0.16 immigrations per unit-year: 37 immigrations during 225 unit-years of solitary males), which is significantly lower than the rate to established groups of one immigration every 2 years (0.41 immigrations per unit-year: 141 immigrations during 340 unit-years of groups) (chi-square = 26.8, df = 1, p < 0.001).

Effect of group size

Females were significantly more likely to emigrate from larger groups when females that disappeared were included (Fig. 3a, model 1 in Tables 3, 4). However, female transfer decisions were not significantly related to the size of the group if those females that disappeared were excluded (S2: Fig. S2a, model 1 in Tables S1, S2). The difference in these two tests may be due to enhanced statistical power from a larger dataset when disappearances were included (365 versus 303 cases). Immigration rates were significantly negatively correlated with group size (Fig. 4a, Table S5), with females more likely joining smaller groups than larger ones. When testing the difference between the group of origin and the group of destination, we found that females transferred significantly more to groups that were smaller than their previous groups; the groups of destination contained an average of four fewer weaned individuals than the origin groups (Fig. 5a, Table S6). Overall, the three different methods show similar results of females transferring to smaller groups.

Fig. 3
figure 3

ac Emigration rate as a function of group size (a) and tenure duration separated into elapsed tenure (b) and remaining tenure (c). The area of the circle depicts the sample size. The dashed line shows the fitted model and the gray area the 95% confidence interval for the prediction. Model results are presented in Tables 3 and 4

Table 4 Emigration rates. Statistical details of generalized linear mixed models testing the effects of male competitive ability, feeding competition, and the death of a previous offspring on female transfer probability using a large dataset including disappearances. p values considered significant (p < 0.05) are in italics
Fig. 4
figure 4

ac Immigration rate as a function of group size (a) and tenure duration separated into elapsed tenure (b) and remaining tenure (c). The area of the circle depicts the sample size. The dashed lines show the fitted model and the 95% confidence interval for the prediction. Statistical details of the linear model in Table S5

Fig. 5
figure 5

ac Difference between destination and origin group. Random intercept models for each separate variable of group size, elapsed tenure, and remaining tenure. The area of the circle depicts the sample size. The results were considered significant when the 97.5% confidence limits did not overlap zero. Statistical details in Table S6

Effect of tenure duration

The female emigration rate was positively correlated with the elapsed male tenure (Fig. 3b, model 2 in Tables 3, 4) and negatively correlated with the remaining male tenure (Fig. 3c, model 3 in Tables 3, 4). Five years after a male tenure started, emigrations accounted for 18% of decisions to stay or leave and increased to 75% of decisions 15 years into the male tenure (Fig. 3b). When looking at the remaining tenure, we found that emigrations accounted for 0% of decisions to stay or leave 15 years prior to the end of a male’s tenure and increased from 35% 5 years before the tenure ended to 80% 1 year before the end (Fig. 3c). These patterns were similar when we excluded the individuals that disappeared (See S2: Fig. S2b, c, models 2, 3 in Tables S1, S2).

The immigration rate was negatively correlated with the elapsed tenure (Fig. 4b) and positively correlated with the remaining tenure (Fig. 4c). The elapsed tenure model showed 1.39 immigrations per group-year for silverbacks early in their tenure (first 0–5 years), which is four times higher than the rate of 0.35 immigrations per group-year in groups with males > 12 years into their tenure. The remaining tenure model showed even a higher difference with 1.47 immigration per group-year in groups with males more than 12 years away from the end of their tenure, which is eight times higher than the rate of 0.18 immigrations per group-year during the last 5 years of the tenure (Table S5).

When comparing the group of destination with the group of origin, we found a trend for a negative effect for the elapsed tenure with females transferring on average to males that were 2 years earlier in their tenure compared to their previous male (Fig. 5b, Table S6). When testing the remaining tenure, we found a significant effect of females transferring to males that were on average 6 years further away from reaching the end of their tenure compared with that of their previous silverback (Fig. 5c, Table S6).

Effect of infant death

The death of an infant had a potential influence on the probability that a female would transfer (statistical trend; model 4 in Tables 3, 4). After having raised an infant to weaning age (N = 55), females conceived in the same group in 76% of the cases and transferred in 24%. After the death of an offspring (N = 58), females were also more likely to stay in the same group (62%) but left more often than after a surviving offspring (38%).

Discussion

Female western lowland gorillas seem to have unconditional natal dispersal, which further supports the inbreeding avoidance hypothesis. Secondary emigration rates increased in response to increasing group size and male tenure duration (Table 1). The rate of female immigration decreased as both group size and male tenure duration increased. These results suggest that secondary dispersal is influenced by a combined pressure of reducing group size, which may reflect lower intra-group feeding competition and/or deflecting attention from outsider males or predators, and residing with males having high competitive ability. The importance of smaller group size for females runs counter to the male interest of having a high number of females highlighting an intersexual conflict found in gorillas.

Unconditional natal dispersal

All female western lowland gorillas transfer before reaching maturity even when they are in a group with a silverback who is not their father (e.g., in a group they joined involuntarily after their group disintegrated) and therefore voluntary dispersal by nulliparous females is unconditional. In species having an adult male tenure longer than the female’s average age at reproduction, it is expected that females leave a group with a putative father to avoid the costs of inbreeding (Clutton-Brock and Lukas 2012). However, it is not clear why females would leave a male that is not their father unless they have enough time before the first conception to find a better male to transfer to or females are socially inhibited from breeding with males who were adults during their maturation (Watts 1990). Non-dispersing natal mountain gorilla females follow this prediction as in all cases of reproduction in a multimale group, the females bred with males who were substantially younger than the dominant male (Vigilant et al. 2015). Mating avoidance with familiar paternal relatives has also been shown in Barbary macaques (Kuester et al. 1994), savannah baboons (Alberts 1999), northern muriquis (Strier et al. 2011), and capuchin monkeys (Muniz et al. 2006). This strategy of unconditional dispersal may be advantageous if females cannot assess relatedness to the silverback of their group especially if the cost of inbreeding is much greater than the cost of dispersal. It is possible that species with high paternity certainty, such as western lowland gorillas living in one-male groups, may not be able to discern kin as well as species that have evolved in multimale groups.

Female preference for smaller groups

Gorillas are expected to have low within-group feeding competition and previous research has resulted in equivocal results for whether feeding competition has a negative effect on female reproductive success or dispersal decisions (Robbins and Robbins 2018). We found the strongest support to date for group size influencing female dispersal decisions in gorillas. However, these results depend partly on whether females that disappeared (unknown if they died or dispersed) are included in the models or not. Females were more likely to emigrate from larger groups and immigrate into small groups, which were on average four individuals smaller than their current group, suggesting within-group feeding competition may be occurring (Stokes 2004; Lodwick 2015). Previous studies did not find a negative relationship between reproductive success and group size, which is not consistent with increased levels of within-group feeding competition in both lowland and mountain gorillas (Robbins et al. 2007; Manguette et al. 2019), but the ability of females to disperse before experiencing any effects of within-group feeding competition may therefore prevent them from suffering reduced fitness (Dias and Strier 2003). This contrasts with female philopatric species which often suffer high reproductive costs of feeding competition in larger groups (e.g., long-tailed macaques, van Noordwijk and van Schaik 1999; yellow baboons, Packer et al. 2000; Phayres leaf monkey, Borries et al. 2008).

Larger gorilla groups with more females could also attract more competitors which may be another argument explaining females’ choice for smaller groups, even in the absence of group take-overs. There is strong evidence that groups are more likely to be targeted for take-overs once female group size becomes larger than optimal (lions: Pusey and Packer 1994; colobus monkeys: Teichroeb et al. 2012). Because female western gorillas only transfer during intergroup encounters, their transfer decisions may therefore also be based on which groups they encounter. Silverbacks leading groups with many reproductive females and/or young infants may try to avoid encounters to reduce the risk of infanticide and their females dispersing. In contrast, young silverbacks with few females in their groups or solitary males may be more motivated to seek out encounters to acquire females (Sicotte 1993). If this is true, then smaller groups should have higher rates of encounters (younger silverbacks and solitary males) and lead to more transfers into them. Furthermore, males use sexual coercion as a means to prevent female transfer, primarily by herding them away from the opposing group and/or exhibiting higher rates of aggression towards those females (Breuer et al. 2016). Females in larger groups at Mbeli Bai received less aggression from the silverbacks than those in smaller groups (Breuer et al. 2016), suggesting that it may be easier to transfer away from groups with more females. Females may also choose smaller groups to reduce harassment by resident females which may be especially true for very young females after an involuntary natal dispersal (Watts 1991).

Female preference for males earlier in their tenure

Females were more likely to join males earlier in their tenures thus males that are younger and potentially stronger. We found females dispersing away from males nearing the end of their tenure (likely aging silverbacks). Male competitive ability therefore seems to be a very important factor influencing female transfer decisions, which is consistent with the idea that females disperse for a better mate who is a better protector (Sicotte 2001). Similar results have been found for another population of western lowland gorillas (Baudouin et al. 2019) and other species including humans (Marsh 1979; Starin 1994; Sterck et al. 1997; Marlowe 2005; Robbins et al. 2009a; Teichroeb et al. 2009). In Thomas langurs, females move from groups with older, long tenure males to groups with younger adult males, resulting in females experiencing fewer potentially infanticidal attacks from extra-group males after transferring (Sterck et al. 1997, 2005). In the Hadza, better hunters have greater reproductive success and younger wives with greater reproductive value (Marlowe 2005; Apicella 2014). Female western lowland gorillas suffer higher infant mortality at the end of a male’s tenure (Manguette et al. 2019). Therefore, leaving a silverback before he reaches the end of his tenure and potentially loses competitive ability or dies seems to be an advantageous strategy to prevent this loss. Seeking males with a better ability to provide protection is also suggested by our observations that transfer rates are slightly higher after the death of an infant, similar to mountain gorillas (Harcourt et al. 1976; Robbins et al. 2009b). The low percentage of female involuntary transfers (17%) is another indication that females typically leave before the death of the silverback. Females may be able to determine that a male is losing strength by using the age of the male, the intensity or duration of their threat displays, or their inability to protect their infant (i.e., following infanticide by extra-group males or predation events).

Low immigration rates to solitary males compared with that to group silverbacks may indicate that solitary silverbacks were possibly of lower quality (e.g., less reproductive and protective value) because they were either too inexperienced to form their own breeding group, old post-reproductive silverbacks, or lower quality male. At Mbeli, silverbacks who never reproduced (N = 2) and post-reproductive silverbacks (N = 4) accounted for 14% (33.5 out of 225.1) of all solitary silverback years indicating that most available solitary males may be too young to form their own breeding groups. Young adult males may have reduced access to mates because of a reduced capacity to protect and monopolize females (Clutton-Brock and McAuliffe 2009). For example, female orangutans avoid mating with young males that are unable to protect them against rivals (Fox 2002) and female elephant seals give calls that attract dominant males if they are courted by younger males (Cox and Le Boeuf 1977). If females use the group composition to evaluate a silverback’s strength, as hypothesized by Baudouin et al. (2019), it may be difficult for a solitary male to initially attract a female and recruitment of additional females may be faster once the first female has joined.

Comparison with other gorilla populations

The emigration rate (proportion of dispersal versus conceiving events) of 33–44% is more than double the 13% rate found for mountain gorillas, although only slightly higher than the 32% emigration rate found when only one-male mountain gorilla groups are considered (Robbins et al. 2009c). Multimale groups are more common in mountain gorillas than in western gorillas and are believed to be a key counterstrategy to infanticide (Robbins and Robbins 2018). The lack of multimale groups in western lowland gorillas may lead to higher female dispersal rates (both voluntary and involuntary) as females have less choice of mates within the group and the silverback is not replaced by a subordinate after his death (as in the case of mountain gorillas) leading to group disintegration and female transfer in western lowland gorillas. The difference in the emigration rate is consistent with the fact that females are less likely to leave multimale groups than one-male groups due to the lower infanticide risk in multimale groups (Robbins et al. 2009c). The immigration rate of 0.314 immigration per group-year in this population is similar to both the female transfer rate reported in another population of western lowland gorilla at Loango in Gabon, of 0.26–0.48 female transfer per group-year (Hagemann et al. 2018) and the 0.27 immigrations per group-year for mountain gorillas in one-male groups (Robbins et al. 2009c). The higher female dispersal rate in western lowland gorillas, which appears to be due to the occurrence of multimale groups in mountain gorillas, may have implications for the differences observed between gorilla species such as lower co-residence with familiar individuals including kin (Watts 2003; Bradley et al. 2007); however, that remains speculative in the absence of genetic data.

While the higher dispersal rate observed in female western lowland gorillas may lead to the low association with kin compared with that in mountain gorillas, western gorilla females have been shown to share relatedness with at least one other female in their group suggesting that dispersal does not prevent certain female kin associations (Bradley et al. 2007; Arandjelovic et al. 2014). The high transfer rate and the fact that the maximum group duration is linked to the duration of a silverback’s life may lead to western gorillas having shorter group duration, average lower female co-residency time, and less incentive to form strong bonds between individuals than mountain gorillas.

Female western lowland gorillas may face more constraints on their group size than mountain gorillas due to more spatially and temporarily clumped fruit in their diet and lower availability of terrestrial herbaceous vegetation (Robbins and Robbins 2018). Western gorillas have longer travel distances and less cohesive groups (Cipolletta 2004; Doran-Sheehy et al. 2004), leading to greater group spread while feeding making it difficult for a single silverback to protect large groups from predation or infanticidal males and therefore constrain female reproductive success at larger group sizes (Tutin 1996; Doran and McNeilage 2001; Salmi and Doran-Sheehy 2014).

Conclusion

Female secondary dispersal is very rare in primates but is common in gorillas. Our results suggest that both group size and male competitive ability play a role, although it is difficult to disentangle the effects of each factor. If several females are attracted to the same male, the female interest in silverbacks early in their tenure may lead to higher mating competition among females (Doran-Sheehy et al. 2009) and higher within-group feeding competition or reduced attraction by outsider males or predators (Harcourt and Stewart 2007). Attractive males very early in their tenure also tend to have smaller groups (Parnell 2002). Therefore, transfer decisions likely are made based on the sum of many factors, with the ultimate aim of maximizing reproductive success (Manguette et al. 2019).

In western lowland gorillas, females suffer sexual coercion by males (Breuer et al. 2016) but the ability to transfer between groups gives females some intersexual power not present in philopatric species (Lewis 2018). Indeed, female dispersal may explain the absence of take-overs in western lowland gorillas. If it was beneficial for females to stay, a take-over male would be guaranteed to keep all the females; however, if residency is not necessarily beneficial to females, the male efforts would be wasted if females abandoned the new silverback as soon as he evicted the resident silverback (Harcourt and Stewart 2007). However, in many female philopatric species, females have developed other counterstrategies to sexual coercion after male take-over such as pregnancy interruption (e.g., baboons, Roberts et al. 2012), infant abandonment (e.g., colobine monkeys, Sicotte et al. 2007), or acceleration of weaning (e.g., vervet monkeys, Fairbanks and McGuire 1987), all strategies which may have stronger deleterious effects on reproductive success than a “planned” secondary dispersal before the death of the silverback.

The high rate of female secondary dispersal in western gorillas coincides with groups being relatively ephemeral because they only exist as long as the silverback has females and/or is alive. This contrasts with female philopatric species and species living in multimale groups that exist for many generations and where females rely on within-group kinship and the ability to form and maintain social bonds. Such long-lasting groups also may be found in multimale species with female dispersal and male philopatry such as chimpanzees (Langergraber et al. 2009; Mitani 2009). It is possible that the high degree of male-male competition in western lowland gorillas not only leads to extreme sexual dimorphism but also influences the formation of this relatively rare social system.