Introduction

Sensorimotor atypicalities are increasingly being viewed as ‘cardinal’ feature of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), which impact on lifelong living proficiencies, social development, and quality of life (Fournier et al. 2010; Gowen and Hamilton 2013). Indeed, movement-related difficulties are experienced by most autistic people (for review, see Gowen and Hamilton 2013), with postural abnormalities, sensory hypersensitivities, and impairments in skills requiring gross and/or fine motor co-ordination all commonplace (Fournier et al. 2010). While these features rarely necessitate medical treatment, they contribute to substantial practical, financial, and health-related hardships (Buescher et al. 2014; Pellicano et al. 2014). For example, movement-based difficulties in autism may underpin reduced motivation and participation in physical activity (Leary and Hill 1996; Scharoun et al. 2017). These difficulties also can precede, and even predict various aptitudes in childhood and adult life (e.g., daily living skills, social skills, Jasmin et al. 2009; Brandwein et al. 2015). Consequently, research into the aetiology and management of these abilities is demanded both by academics (Gowen and Hamilton 2013) and the autism community (Pellicano et al. 2014).

Emerging research suggests that these sensorimotor difficulties stem from atypical predictive processing, with autistic people proposed to utilise prior information less accurately and/or efficiently (e.g., Pellicano and Burr 2012; Gomot and Wicker 2012; Friston et al. 2013; Sinha et al. 2014; Van de Cruys et al. 2014). Sensorimotor control involve complex, co-ordinated contributions from various distinct subcomponents (e.g., cognitive, visual, and motor systems), which respond to ‘bottom–up’ (stimulus-driven) informational sources and internally driven (‘top–down’) predictive models (Corbetta et al. 2008; Land 2009). Abnormalities in ‘top–down’ control can limit the performance and learning of goal-directed actions (Kording et al. 2007; Land 2009) and may exemplify a ‘shared endophenotype’ that underpins socio-behavioral difficulties in autism (e.g., social-communication deficits, repetitive behaviors, and attention to detail; Pellicano and Burr 2012; Sinha et al. 2014). Indeed, in motor control studies, autistic individuals show impaired postural adjustments in anticipation of changes in object load (Schmitz et al. 2003) and inaccurate initial force outputs during precision-grip actions (Mosconi et al. 2013; Wang et al. 2015), effects which signal an increased reliance on ‘bottom–up’ (as opposed to ‘top–down’) sensory information. Similarly, prediction-related differences emerge in cognition and visual processing, with autistic individuals demonstrating diminished ‘top–down’ gaze adaptation in double-step saccade paradigms (Johnson et al. 2013; Mosconi et al. 2013) and abnormalities in prediction-related neural regions (e.g., the cerebellum, Frith 2003). Such ‘top–down’ limitations lead to greater employment of ‘bottom–up’ attentional (e.g., proprioception, visual feedback; Haswell et al. 2009) and neurobiological systems (e.g., Soulières et al. 2009), while co-vary with movement-related difficulties in autism (Mosconi et al. 2013).

However, feedforward atypicalities have not been consistently detected in research (Palmer et al. 2017; Tewolde et al. 2018). For example, autistic children exhibit typical rates of motor adaptation in various tasks that require, and depend on, broad abilities to utilise ‘top–down’ internal models (Gidley-Larson et al. 2008), while prediction-related atypicalities in perception (e.g., global processing; Brosnan et al. 2004) do not inevitably transfer onto action or behavior (Palmer et al. 2017). Similarly, the nature and severity of movement-related difficulties varies between individuals and empirical contexts (Green et al. 2002; Palmer et al. 2017). This has prompted suggestions that autism-related difficulties originate from finer, context-sensitive differences in the integration of predictive and environmental statistics (Lawson et al. 2014; Palmer et al. 2017), as opposed to generic attenuations in the use of prior expectations. Consequently, research must decipher which specific mechanisms are implicated in autism (Haker et al. 2016). To do this, illusion-based paradigms offer notable value, as they can highlight ‘top–down’ influences on the processing of ambiguous sensory information (Geisler and Kersten 2002; Brown and Friston 2012). Interestingly, although autistic people do appear less susceptible to some perceptual illusions (e.g., Happé 1996; Mitchell et al. 2010; Ropar and Mitchell 2002), results are mixed and often complicated by heterogeneity in sampling characteristics (Van der Hallen et al. 2015).

To address these empirical inconsistencies and better separate autism-specific atypicalities from potential confounds (e.g., cognitive ability, symptom severity, development, and comorbidities), recent research has explored how sensorimotor outcomes relate to autistic-like traits in general populations (Landry and Chouinard 2016). Autistic-like traits are behavioral characteristics such as social imperviousness, directness in conversation, lack of imagination, affinity for solitude, and difficulty displaying emotions (Gernsbacher et al. 2017), which can be readily indexed using self-report measures such as the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ: Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). Such autistic-like traits vary continuously across the general population, with ASD proposed to reside at the extreme end of this continuum (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001, 2006; Ruzich et al. 2015). Consequently, empirical links between self-reported autistic-like traits and behavioral variables have enabled researchers to identify various cognitive, perceptual, and social differences associated with autism (e.g., Almeida et al. 2014; Poljac et al. 2013; Cooper et al. 2013; Jameel et al. 2014).

Interestingly, higher levels of autistic-like traits have been shown to relate to reduced illusory effects in some non-clinical studies (e.g., Chouinard et al. 2013, 2016). Recently, from a sensorimotor perspective, Buckingham et al. (2016) explored links between autistic-like traits and predictive sensorimotor control during object lifting, using a Size–Weight Illusion (SWI) paradigm. In the SWI, small objects are experienced as feeling heavier more than larger ones of an equal mass (Charpentier 1891), an effect underpinned by the prior expectation that larger items tend to be heavier than smaller items (Buckingham 2014). Interestingly, no relationship emerged between autistic-like traits and the magnitude of this illusion, challenging the assumptions of broad autism-related atypicalities in prediction (e.g., Pellicano and Burr 2012). However, participants with higher levels of autistic-like traits showed reduced ‘top–down’ bias of movement, as indexed by differences in peak grip and load force rates between larger (heavy-looking) and smaller (lighter-looking) objects. These findings suggest that, despite being equally susceptible to the perceptual SWI, high-trait individuals are less inclined to utilise prior information in their motor programmes, a dissociation which has also been reported for the rubber-hand illusion (Palmer et al. 2013, 2015).

The transferability of these results across movement-based contexts remains unclear, as observed relationships were weak (R2 = 0.06) and likely dependent on contextual factors. On one hand, ‘top–down’ expectations of weight influence lifting forces when objects differ in material, shape, and/or density (Gordon et al. 1991; Grandy and Westwood 2006; Buckingham et al. 2009). Similarly, abilities to regulate grip forces are influential in various daily living skills, including those known to be impaired in autism (e.g., dressing and writing; Fuentes et al. 2009; Wang et al. 2015). Conversely though, Buckingham et al. (2016)’s results may not necessarily reflect gross attenuations in the use of prior information, as lifting actions are directed by various cognitive (e.g., expected weight; Johansson and Westling 1988), attentional (e.g., vision; Gordon et al. 1991), and haptic (e.g., density; Grandy and Westwood 2006) mechanisms. Moreover, it is argued that something is unique about how volumetric features are processed in the brain (Saccone and Chouinard 2019), with the SWI underpinned by context-specific ‘top–down’ expectancies (i.e., predictions related to size-weight modelling; Buckingham and Goodale 2013) and haptic cues (e.g., object density; Buckingham 2014). As these processing tendencies are not entirely dependent on prior experience or knowledge (Saccone and Chouinard 2019), further scrutiny into the observed effects is warranted.

Therefore, we utilised a Material–Weight illusion (MWI) paradigm to better isolate associations between autistic-like traits and predictive sensorimotor control. Like the SWI, the MWI occurs when heavy-looking materials (e.g., granite) are perceived as feeling lighter, and lifted with greater initial force rates, than lighter-looking (e.g., polystyrene) items of the same mass (Wolfe 1898; Seashore 1899; Buckingham et al. 2009). Importantly, these effects are not driven by size-based expectations or low-level haptic cues (e.g., variations in centre of mass or density), but by prior expectations relating to material properties derived from prior experiences (Saccone and Chouinard 2019). Consequently, in line with predictive theories of autism (e.g., Pellicano and Burr 2012; Sinha et al. 2014) and previous illusory research (e.g., Chouinard et al. 2013), we hypothesised that the number of autistic-like traits that an individual presents will negatively correlate with the magnitude of the perceptual MWI.

Beyond our examination of fingertip forces, we conducted a multi-modal assessment of sensorimotor control to explore whether any abnormalities are broad and transferable across processing domains (Pellicano and Burr 2012), or whether they are underpinned by precise mechanisms (e.g., relating to environmental volatility, Lawson et al. 2017). Specifically, to extend Buckingham et al. (2016)’s previous findings, we probed expectation-related changes in both lifting forces and velocities between light- (polystyrene) and heavy-looking (granite) materials. Here, attenuations in ‘top–down’ control can be signalled via less-divergent lifting profiles (i.e., reduced expectation-based scaling of movement; Johansson and Westling 1988) and prolonged preparatory movements phases, which facilitate proprioceptive (i.e., ‘bottom–up’) interpretations of object mass (Hamilton et al. 2007). We also measured visual search rate and gaze fixations, as longer fixations prior to skill execution reflect extended periods of ‘top–down’ cognitive processing (Vickers 1996) and increases in search rate (i.e., shorter, more-frequent fixations) signal more stimulus-driven attentional control (Williams et al. 2002; Corbetta et al. 2008). On the basis of the aforementioned ‘domain-general’ theories (Pellicano and Burr 2012; Sinha et al. 2014) and Buckingham et al. (2016)’s data, which posit that socio-behavioral and movement-based difficulties in autism are both underpinned by atypical predictive processing, we estimated that ‘top–down’ sensorimotor control would be correlated with self-reported autistic-like traits. Specifically, greater autistic-like traits were hypothesised to co-vary with a reduced susceptibility to the perceptual MWI, attenuated expectation-based scaling of lifting force rate, prolonged preparatory movement kinematics, elevated visual search rates, and shorter gaze fixations prior to skill execution.

Methods

Participants

Ninety-two participants (47 males and 45 females; 23.10 ± 3.32 years) were recruited, the majority of whom (n = 83; 90%) were self-reported right handers. All were naïve to the study aims and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Participants reporting any condition known to affect sensorimotor control, including ASD, were excluded. One individual with developmental co-ordination disorder and one with prior injury were removed. The study received approval from the School of Sport and Health Sciences Ethics Committee (University of Exeter) and informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Materials

To measure autistic-like traits, participants completed the 50-item adult Autistic Quotient (AQ: Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). The AQ assesses five sub-traits associated with ASD: attention to detail, attention switching, imagination, communication, and social skills. Participants self-reported on a four-point Likert scale, signalling whether they “definitely agree”, “slightly agree”, “slightly disagree”, or “definitely disagree” with 50 itemised statements assessing each subscale. Example statements include “I enjoy social occasions” (social skills), “I tend to notice details that others do not” (attentional switching) and “I am fascinated by dates” (attention to detail). The measure has proven reliable and valid for research use in general populations (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001; Woodbury-Smith et al. 2005), providing an overall score out of 50, whereby higher numbers reflect greater autistic tendencies. A score of 32 was proposed as a threshold above which seeking a diagnosis would be recommended for people who thought that they might be autistic (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). As such, to reduce the possibility of relationships being driven by clinically related confounding factors (e.g., cognitive ability, symptom severity, development; Landry and Chouinard 2016), participants who recorded above this value were excluded from statistical analysis after they had completed the study (as in Buckingham et al. 2016).

Participants were then presented with three identically sized (5 × 5 × 5 cm) cubes with different surface materials (Fig. 1a), namely: granite (unaltered density: 2.6 g/cm3), corkwood (unaltered density: 0.25 g/cm3), and expanded polystyrene (unaltered density: 0.03 g/cm3). Specifically, polystyrene (i.e., light-looking) and granite (i.e., heavy-looking) were used to elicit the MWI, whereas corkwood was selected to provide a ‘control’ object which was markedly closer to its natural (i.e., expected) weight. Each of the surface materials was sealed around a hollow wooden box, filled with lead shot and putty to provide a weight of 230 g. A clear adhesive was used to seal the surface material to its inner structure, thereby making the object appear completely made from its visible outer material. Care was taken to ensure that the centre of mass coincided with each object’s geometric centre.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The expanded polystyrene, corkwood, and granite objects lifted by participants (a) and the experimental set-up during a lifting trial (b)

A mount was positioned on each object’s top surface to facilitate lifting. Attached to this mount was an ATI Nano-17 Force transducer fitted within an aluminum and plastic handle (Fig. 1b), which recorded forces in three dimensions at 500 Hz. Grip force was defined by forces orthogonal to the handle’s surface, whereas load forces were yielded from the vector sum of the remaining values. Four reflective markers were attached to the object handle to create a detectable rigid body, which was tracked at 120 Hz by infrared cameras using motion-capture technology (OptiTrack Flex13, NaturalPoint, Corvallis, Oregon). Four markers were also positioned on a ‘lifting glove’ (Fig. 1b), which was worn on the dominant hand of participants to track hand movements.Footnote 1 Participants were fitted with a Pupil Labs mobile eye-tracking system (Pupil Labs, Sanderstrasse, Berlin, Germany; Kassner et al. 2014), a pair of lightweight glasses (34 g) which collates information from scene and infrared eye cameras to calculate gaze positions at 90 Hz (spatial accuracy of ± 0.60° of visual angle; 0.08° precision). Prior to lifting procedures, the eye-tracking system was calibrated using the manufacturers built-in screen marker routine (Pupil Labs 2016), which was presented upon a large LED screen (60.96 cm; Dell Computer Corporation, Round Rock, TX, USA) that spanned the entire lifting workspace.Footnote 2 Calibration procedures were repeated upon any displacement of gaze cameras. A chin rest was attached to the table to restrict head movements and a manual clapper board concealed objects before trials.

Procedure

Participants first completed the AQ before undertaking the lifting protocol, consisting of 5 baseline lifts and 24 subsequent trials. Participants were seated throughout these trials, with their head positioned upon the chin rest, and were instructed to start with their dominant hand positioned to the side of the object. Each object was placed quietly in front of participants and concealed behind a closed clapper board until the onset of each trial, so that there was no prior indication of their properties. Upon a computer-generated auditory tone, the manual clapper board was opened to reveal an object, and participants reached out to grasp the lifting handle with their thumb and forefinger of their dominant hand. Participants were instructed to vertically lift the object in a ‘smooth, controlled, and confident manner’ at a self-selected speed, before holding it steady ‘a few centimetres above the table’. Upon a second auditory tone (+ 4 s after trial onset), they were required to gently place the object back in its starting position, before verbally reporting a numerical judgement about how heavy it felt. Apart from the condition, that larger numbers should represent higher weights, no constraints or ranges were placed on this measure so as to minimise biases associated with ratio scaling (Zwislocki and Goodman 1980).

Instructions of these standardised procedures were given ahead of the lifting protocol. Thereafter, the corkwood object was lifted five times, with participants informed that the object would not change during these baseline lifts. No procedural errors were displayed by participants during baseline lifts 3–5, suggesting that they were all familiar with the task requirements. Subsequent MWI trials consisted of lifting each object eight times, presenting a total of 24 lifts. The object used in each trial was determined from a completely randomised order, which was newly formulated for each participant to account for any potential order effects on weight perception (Maiello et al. 2018). Upon completion of all procedures, participants were verbally debriefed.

Data analysis

Perceived heaviness scores

Self-ratings for each lift were normalised to a z score distribution to provide a measure of perceived heaviness. To quantify the magnitude of the experienced MWI, average values for the heavier-looking (granite) objects were subtracted from those of the lighter-looking (expanded polystyrene) objects (as in Buckingham et al. 2009, 2016).

Force data

Extracted data from the force transducers were processed and analyzed using a custom algorithm in MATLAB. Data were first smoothed using a 14-Hz dual-pass Butterworth filter, with forces perpendicular to the surface of the handle defined as grip force and resultant vectors of the tangential forces interpreted as load force (all as in Buckingham et al. 2009, 2016). To determine the rates of change, data were differentiated with a five-point central difference equation, with the maximum values on the initial lift for each trial determining peak grip (pGFR) and load (pLFR) force rates. Force rates from the first lift, as opposed to averages from all trials, were analyzed, as lifting forces adapt rapidly over repeated lifts (Flanagan and Beltzner 2000; Buckingham et al. 2009). To provide an index of prediction-led motor bias, grip (pGFRdiff) and load (pLFRdiff) force rates utilised in the first lift of the polystyrene object were subtracted from those of the granite object. Here, values from the first lift, as opposed to averages from across all lifts, were analyzed, as differences in lifting forces diminish rapidly over repeated lifts (Flanagan and Beltzner 2000; Buckingham et al. 2009).Footnote 3 For these index scores, greater values would signify greater utility of feedforward information at a motor level.

Kinematic data

Positional data for each rigid body were smoothed using a dual-pass, zero-phase lag Butterworth filter at 10 Hz (the ‘optimum’ cut-off frequency reported for upper-limb movement control research; Franks et al. 1990). Hand and object velocities were calculated from the average position of their respected rigid bodies. We then segmented trials into four distinct phases: Reach, Grasp, Transport, and Hold (as in Lavoie et al. 2018). The reach phase started when hand velocity first exceeded 50 mm/s for three consecutive frames (Eastough and Edwards 2007) and concluded upon the onset of grip force (i.e., the Grasp phase). The Lift phase was then determined from the first timepoint whereby both Hand and Object velocity exceeded 50 mm/s. Finally, the Hold phase was derived from the timepoint where the object reached its maximum vertical position (endpoint of Lift phase) until the onset of the second auditory tone (trial completion). Total movement time was calculated from the onset of Reach to the offset of the Hold phase. The duration of each phase was recorded for baseline lifts and for the first lift of each MWI-inducing object. Furthermore, maximum velocity of the hand during reach (MRV) and lift (MLV) phases was recorded, as were the timepoints where this occurred (as a % of total movement time).

Gaze data

Visual fixations were extracted from gaze data using Pupil Player software (Pupil Labs 2016). Fixations were defined as a gaze that remained on a location (within 1° visual angle) for a minimum of 120 ms, with the total number and average duration of fixations recorded. To quantify visual search rate, the number of fixations was divided by the average fixation duration. To index ‘top–down’ control, we calculated the Quiet Eye (QE) duration, which represents the final fixation or tracking gaze before the initiation of a planned motor response (Vickers 1996). This was operationalised as the final fixation or tracking gaze directed to any single location in the workspace within 3° of visual angle (of the normalised position of the fixation’s centroid) for a minimum of 100 ms prior to the onset of the lift phase. These variables were assessed for baseline trials and for the first lift of each MWI object. Longer QE durations signify greater ‘top–down’ processing (Vine et al. 2014), whereas higher search rates are indicative of more stimulus-driven attention (Corbetta et al. 2008).

Eye–hand integration

To index the integration between gaze and kinematic outcomes, cross-correlational analysis (based on Chattington et al. 2007) explored the corresponding signals for the changes in the vertical component of eye and hand movement. First, positional hand data were resampled at 90 Hz, via interpolation, and gaze data were smoothed using a dual-pass, zero-phase lag Butterworth filter at 45-Hz (i.e., a low-pass cut-off deemed appropriate for detecting saccadic eye movements; Bahill et al. 1981). Thereafter, the two signals were manually synchronised for time, using detectable landmarks in the motion-capture and eye-tracking footage. Specifically, the frame denoting the onset of the reach movement was visually detected in the raw gaze data, before being aligned with the corresponding frame in the motion-capture data (i.e., where hand velocity first exceeds 50 mm/s for three consecutive frames). As the synchronised signals followed notably comparable profiles during the grasp, lift, and hold movements (see Online Appendix 1), data were then segmented from the start of the grasp phase (i.e., the timepoint corresponding to the onset of grip force) to the offset of the hold phase (i.e., the timepoint where the ‘object’ rigid body reached its maximum vertical position). The resulting cross-correlogram identified the peak covariation of the two signals (i.e., peak R) and the ‘lag’ (converted into time) for when this peak covariation occurred. This ‘lag’ measure quantified the degree to which one signal may lead another, with lower (i.e., more negative values), signifying that eye movements were preceding the hand to a greater extent in a more feedforward manner. This provided further insight into whether systems are integrated in a ‘top–down’ or ‘bottom–up’ manner (Chattington et al. 2007).

Preliminary analysis

Patterns of missing and complete values were identified for all data and the probability of these patterns diverging from randomness was estimated using Little’s MCAR test. To assist missing value analysis, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients assessed the reliability of AQ subscales. Outliers were inspected for all variables and, where detected, removed from their respected analysis (as recommended by Osbourne 2013). Here, univariate outliers were identified as values > 3.29 SD above or below the mean (p < 0.001) and multivariate outliers ascertained by extreme Mahalanobis distances (p < 0.001; Tabachnick and Fidell 2007). Participants with > 10% of data identified as ‘missing’ or ‘outliers’ were excluded from analysis. For all variables, normality of data was examined from z scores for skewness and kurtosis, while assumptions relating to linearity, homoscedasticity, and multicollinearity were inspected from correlation matrices and scatterplots of residuals (Garson 2012).

Statistical analysis

To assess whether participants experienced the MWI and showed prediction-related motor patterns, separate three (polystyrene, corkwood, granite) × 8 (trials 1–8) repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted, with pGFR, pLFR, and heaviness scores entered as dependent variables. Planned t tests using the Bonferroni correction probed significant effects, with effect sizes calculated using partial-eta squared (\(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\)). Pearson’s correlation examined relationships between AQ scores, perceptual MWI index scores and prediction-related measures of force (pGFRdiff, pLFRdiff), movement (grasp phase duration, MRV, MLV, and time to maximum velocity), gaze (search rate, QE duration), and eye–hand ‘lag’. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 25.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA), with significance accepted for all the tests at p < 0.05 and data presented ± SD.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Incomplete cases were inferred as missing completely at random on the basis of Little’s MCAR test (p > 0.05), while Cronbach’s alpha coefficients indicated that AQ subscales were highly reliable (α > 0.70; Nunnally 1978). Consequently, missing AQ items (0.04%) were replaced using scale mean imputation and participants (n = 4) with > 10% of incomplete data were excluded from analysis. Three further participants were excluded due to “clinically significant” AQ scores (> 32) affording a final sample of 83. Remaining AQ scores ranged from 5 to 32 (Mean: 15.98 ± 6.60) and are, thus, comparable to Buckingham et al. (2016) previous data set (Mean: 15.41 ± 6.09). For sensorimotor outcomes, six participants were removed from force analysis (remaining n = 77), due to equipment malfunction and/or outlier analysis, while four participants were removed from kinematic analysis (remaining n  = 79) and twenty from gaze analysis (remaining n  = 63) due to poor data quality. There were no statistical violations relating to normality, homoscedasticity, and linearity observed on the remaining data. Mauchly’s test indicated that pGFR and pLFR violated the assumptions of sphericity (p < 0.05) and the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied. No further modification or exclusion of variables was necessary. None of the perceptual or sensorimotor variables were significantly different between genders (all p > 0.12) or left and right handers (p’s > 0.15; as in Buckingham et al. 2012).

Primary analysis

Perceptual MWI

ANOVA revealed that a robust MWI was induced (Fig. 2A), with effects of material on perceived heaviness evident (F(2, 162) = 59.57, p < 0.001, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.42). Average scores for the polystyrene object were greater than corkwood values (t(82) = 5.42, p < 0.001), which, in turn, were significantly greater than those reported for the granite object (t(82) = 5.38, p < 0.001). Surprisingly, a ‘material-by-trial’ interaction also emerged (F(10.91, 883.43) = 3.54, p < 0.001, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.04), with the magnitude of the illusion greater on the initial lift of each object (Fig. 2a). Nevertheless, differences between materials were present during both initial (F(2, 164) = 59.59, p < 0.001, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.40) and final (F(2,164) = 24.05, p < 0.001, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.23) trials, suggesting that the MWI remained over the protocol. However, no significant associations between AQ scores and the magnitude of this effect emerged (R = 0.11; p = 0.34; Fig. 3a), indicating that autistic-like traits are unrelated to one’s experience of this perceptual illusion.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Trial-by-trial averages (± SEM) for normalised perceived heaviness ratings (a), peak grip force rate (pGRF; b), and peak load force rate (pLFR; c) across all trials

Fig. 3
figure 3

Scatter plots highlighting associations between AQ scores and the magnitude of the SWI (a), pGFRdiff (b), and pLFRdiff (c). No significant relationships emerged (all p > 0.05)

Sensorimotor control

ANOVA revealed significant effects of object material on pGFR (F(2,148) = 35.298, p < 0.001, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.32) and pLFR (F(2,144) = 18.09, p < 0.001, \(\eta_{\text{p}}^{2}\) = 0.20). As displayed in Fig. 2, fingertip forces were lower on the first trial when lifting the polystyrene box compared to when lifting the corkwood (pGFR: mean difference = 3.76 ± 8.05 N/s; t(76) = 4.10, p < 0.001; pLFR: mean difference = 2.18 ± 4.01 N/s; t(77) = 4.81, p < 0.001) and granite (pGFR: mean difference = 10.04 ± 14.10 N/s; t(77) = 6.29, p < 0.001; pLFR: mean difference = 3.31 ± 5.73 N/s; t(76) = 5.06, p < 0.001) objects. Similarly, grip forces used to lift the granite box were significantly greater than those used to grip the corkwood one (pGFR: mean difference = 6.25 ± 12.33 N/s; t(76) = 4.45, p < 0.001), although pLFR were not significantly different between these objects (t(76) = 1.05, p = 0.05). As expected, prediction-led biases in fingertip forces reduced over the lifting protocol (Fig. 2), suggesting that sensorimotor adaptation occurred. Therefore, these force data indicated that material-related weight expectancies biased motor control, particularly on the initial lifts of each object. However, pGFRdiff and pLFRdiff values were not significantly related to AQ scores (both p > 0.15; Fig. 3), suggesting that this generic predictive bias of motor control is not linked to autistic tendencies. Furthermore, no significant relationships emerged between AQ scores and any gaze or kinematic indicators of predictive control (all p > 0.12).

Exploratory analysis

Naturally, effective predictive control of perception and action is dependent on accurate representations of environmental statistics (Friston 2005; Bastos et al. 2012), with ‘bottom–up’ attentional systems activated when uncertainty about one’s beliefs is high (Yu and Dayan 2003). However, recent theory (e.g., Lawson et al. 2014, 2017; Palmer et al. 2017) suggests that feedforward atypicalities in autism may arise from abnormalities in such processing. Therefore, given the null associations observed between autistic-like traits and broad indices of predictive control, we explored finer mechanisms relating to the context-sensitive integration of prior information and environmental statistics. Specifically, we indexed the degree to which AQ scores co-vary with uncertainty-related adjustments in gaze control, through subtracting average search rates in the final three baseline trials (i.e., where object properties were familiar and the likelihood of unexpected outcomes was minimal) from the first lift of each MWI object (i.e., where probabilistic and environmental statistics were uncertain).Footnote 4

As expected, search rate increased between baseline and MWI lifts (t(62) = 4.24, p < 0.001), an effect driven by shorter fixation durations (average change: − 0.07 ± 0.13 s) which indicated that ‘bottom–up’ attentional systems were generally activated in uncertain trials (Fig. 4a). Interestingly though, these context-sensitive increases (i.e., differences in search rate between baseline and high-uncertainty trials) were negatively correlated with AQ scores (R = − 0.32, p = 0.01), with more pronounced changes in low-trait compared to high-trait participants (Fig. 4b). This suggests that the utility of ‘top–down’ information was less flexible in those with greater autistic-like traits (Lawson et al. 2017).

Fig. 4
figure 4

a Changes in search rate (± SEM) from baseline (lifts 3–5) to uncertain (initial MWI lifts for each object) lifts, and b scatter plot highlighting the relationship between AQ scores and the magnitude of these changes. *Denotes significant difference (p < 0.05)

Finally, given the possibility that some autistic-like traits may be more closely related to predictive processing than others, we explored relationships between individual AQ subscales and each of the sensorimotor outcomes included in the primary analysis (see Online Appendix 2 for Table). In line with our main findings, no associations emerged for any of force, kinematic, gaze, or perceptual variables (all p > 0.08), reinforcing observations that broad sensorimotor prediction is unrelated to autistic-like traits in the context of the MWI. Eye–hand ‘lag’ was observed to weakly correlate with ‘attention to detail’ (R = 0.26, p = 0.045) and ‘attention switching’ (R = 0.28, p = 0.03) subscales though, suggesting that there may be an association between visuomotor integration and autistic-like attentional traits.

Discussion

In this study, we explored links between autistic-like traits and predictive sensorimotor control in a non-clinical population. To do this, we employed an MWI paradigm, whereby the apparent materials of identically sized and weighted objects were manipulated to elicit prediction-related patterns of perception, gaze, and movement. Manipulation checks indicated that prior expectations of object weight biased both perception and action (Fig. 2), permitting scrutiny into whether prediction-related tendencies are inherently related to autistic-like traits (e.g., as proposed by Pellicano and Burr 2012; Gomot and Wicker 2012; Friston et al. 2013; Sinha et al. 2014; Van de Cruys et al. 2014).

Contrary to our hypotheses, AQ scores were unrelated to the magnitude of the perceptual MWI (Fig. 3a), suggesting that the influence of prior knowledge on weight perception was comparable for individuals from across the general autism phenotype. These findings are difficult to reconcile with some predictive theories of autism (e.g., Pellicano and Burr 2012; Sinha et al. 2014) and illusory-based perceptual research (e.g., Happé 1996), as participants with greater AQ scores were equally susceptible to these prediction-led biases. However, they align with the null relationships observed between autistic-like traits and most classical illusions (Chouinard et al. 2016). In particular, our results indicate that the null perceptual effects observed by Buckingham et al. (2016) in the SWI were not specific to size processing mechanisms and hold true across a range of prior expectations.

Furthermore, and again contrary to our initial hypotheses, no broad-scale abnormalities in ‘top–down’ control of action were detected in high-trait individuals. Specifically, the extent to which prediction influenced motor patterns and gaze behaviors was unrelated to AQ scores, despite the previous findings that high-trait individuals utilise prior information differently in lifting motor programmes (Buckingham et al. 2016). Instead, participants generally displayed classic lifting profiles, irrespective of their AQ scores, whereby heavy-looking items were lifted with higher force rates than lighter-looking ones (Fig. 2b, c; Gordon et al. 1991; Flanagan and Beltzner 2000). Although seemingly contradictive of various sensorimotor research (e.g., Mosconi et al. 2013; Buckingham et al. 2016), this corresponds with a meaningful body of clinical evidence which has shown broad prediction-dependent capabilities to be typical in autistic people (Mostofsky et al. 2004; Gidley-Larson et al. 2008; Ego et al. 2016; Tewolde et al. 2018). Findings also align with recent evidence that autistic and neurotypical individuals attend to similar information when presented with visual illusions (Chouinard et al. 2018). Consequently, in contrast to broad predictive accounts of autism (e.g., Pellicano and Burr 2012; Sinha et al. 2014), our data indicate that links between sensorimotor prediction and autistic-like traits may not be due to any generic processing abnormalities, but rather due to context-sensitive ‘high-level’ mechanisms.

Recent theories propose that autistic-like traits may relate to finer mechanisms involved in the context-sensitive adjustment of ‘top–down’ and ‘bottom–up’ control systems (Lawson et al. 2014, 2017; Palmer et al. 2017). These contemporary accounts argue that prediction-related atypicalities may arise from implicit tendencies to misinterpret the uncertainty of an environment, with perception and action resting on internal representations of volatility (Friston 2005; Bastos et al. 2012). Typically, under more volatile conditions, less predictive attentional patterns emerge (Vossel et al. 2013), as evident in our data, where search rate generally increased between baseline and uncertain trials (Fig. 4a). This suppression of ‘top–down’ control is often adaptive, as prior expectations are less reliable in uncertain environments (Brown and Friston 2012), and resultant elevations in neural gain facilitate learning (Burge et al. 2008; Kording et al. 2007). Interestingly though, context-sensitive changes in search rate were reduced in high-trait participants (Fig. 4b), suggesting that the dynamic integration of prior information and environmental statistics may be decreased in these individuals. Although novel, such data are consistent with perceptual research, where high-trait participants showed reduced distinction between low- and high-volatility conditions (Lawson et al. 2017). They also correspond with the recent observations in the rubber-hand illusion, where participants with greater autistic-like traits displayed reduced uncertainty-related slowing of movement, despite experiencing typical perceptual effects (Palmer et al. 2013, 2015). Taken together, these results support proposals that predictive atypicalities in autism may stem from misrepresentations of environmental uncertainty (Lawson et al. 2014, 2017).

These contemporary explanations account for why feedforward differences are shown in some, but not all empirical paradigms, as environmental statistics will naturally vary. For example, it is plausible that context-sensitive representations of uncertainty differed in the present MWI study from Buckingham et al. (2016) SWI protocol, where the congruity between expected and actual weight will have differed. Furthermore, given the “finer”, “context-sensitive” predictive processes implicated by these theoretical frameworks (Palmer et al. 2017, p 521), quantifiable differences in sensorimotor control are unlikely to transfer across SWI and MWI lifting paradigms, as they are underpinned by different mechanisms (Buckingham 2014; Saccone and Chouinard 2019). Nevertheless, various autism-related movement difficulties can be explained by heightened perceptions of volatility, with motor skill performance (Land 2009) and adaptation (Burge et al. 2008) both impaired by contextually inappropriate weightings of ‘top–down’ and ‘bottom–up’ control. Therefore, given the growing evidence for these explanations, research should explore the effects of environmental volatility on sensorimotor control in autism. The use of weight-based illusions to further this understanding remains profitable, as they facilitate holistic exploration of sensorimotor control in a manner that is not contingent upon communicative or motivational competencies (Fisk and Goodale 1989).

Currently, our findings must be interpreted with caution in the context of clinical populations (Gregory and Plaisted-Grant 2016), with inferences essentially indirect at this stage (Skewes et al. 2015). Although trait-based approaches are advocated in the recent research (Chouinard et al. 2013), motor impairments are more prevalent and/or severe in clinical populations (Green et al. 2002) and may, thus, differ in aetiology. Further research is consequently required to examine whether results hold in individuals with clinically diagnosed ASD, to assist in the development of evidence-based practical interventions that are warranted by autistic stakeholders and representative organisations (Pellicano et al. 2014; Myers and Johnson 2007). Indeed, it is argued that greater scrutiny into prediction-related mechanisms, such as those discussed in here, could present numerous avenues for prospective diagnostic and treatment programmes (see Haker et al. 2016 for detailed discussion). Though it must be emphasised that our study provides only a tentative starting point in this research development, it is hoped that future work will be directed towards helping autistic people “manage themselves with whatever difficulties they have” (Pellicano et al. 2014, p 6).

It must also be noted that the simplistic nature of our motor task may limit the validity of ‘eye–hand’ measures. As the goal of each trial was to assess object weight, deviations in ‘top–down’ and ‘bottom–up’ mechanisms were difficult to detect, with the objects providing an informational source for both attentional systems. Thus, unsurprisingly, ‘eye–hand’ lag times (0.23 ± 0.09) were temporally closer than those previously observed (e.g., Lavoie et al. 2018), as gaze tended to follow the object in a manner that aids perception of weight (Hamilton et al. 2007). Interestingly, sub-trait analysis (Online Appendix 2) suggested that this integration of visuomotor systems may be related to autistic-like attentional traits. This exploratory link evidently requires further empirical scrutiny, with heightened perceptions of volatility proposed to disrupt the ‘connectivity’ of neurobiological systems (Friston et al. 2013). More sophisticated eye–hand analysis is warranted during tasks with an external goal (e.g., Lavoie et al. 2018), where eye movements typically precede those of the hand in an empirically quantifiable fashion (Chattington et al. 2007). Such enquiry could improve our understanding of how sensory domains might be related in autism (Robertson and Baron-Cohen 2017).

Overall, our findings suggest that sensorimotor atypicalities in people with greater autistic tendencies do not originate from domain-general processing impairments, but rather from specific differences in the utility of predictive control. Participants with greater autistic-like traits appeared equally susceptible to predictive biases elicited by the MWI at multiple sensorimotor levels. However, these individuals showed reduced context-sensitive adjustments in gaze control under uncertain conditions, supporting links between autistic-like traits and inflexible representations of environmental volatility. Research is required to further our mechanistic understanding of these effects and enable the development of effective evidence-based strategies for the autism community.