Skip to main content
Log in

Hazard levels of warning signal words modulate the inhibition of return effect: evidence from the event-related potential P300

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Erratum to this article was published on 26 March 2016

Abstract

Warning signal words are often used to convey valuable information about potential dangers in everyday life. In this study, we explored whether and how the hazard level of warning signal words modulated participants’ attention to subsequent targets. Event-related potentials with high temporal resolution were employed in a cue–target paradigm. In this task, warning signal words with different hazard levels were used as cues. Participants were required to judge whether targets were presented on the screen horizontally or vertically. We found an inhibition of return (IOR) effect, i.e., participants had longer reaction times to validly cued targets than to invalidly cued targets. Accordingly, the IOR effect was reflected by a smaller P300 amplitude for invalidly cued targets compared to validly cued targets. Furthermore, the IOR effect was eliminated when the cues were high-hazard words. The dampening effect on the P300 was eliminated when the cues were high-hazard warning signal words. The lack of an IOR was attributed to participants’ attentional bias to high-hazard stimuli, which are difficult for participants to disengage their attention from. The current study suggests that warning signal words are a particular type of stimulus that can override the IOR effect. Warning signal words with a high hazard level are more effective in successfully alerting people to risk in a hazardous environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams A, Bochner S, Bilik L (1998) The effectiveness of warning signs in hazardous work places: cognitive and social determinants. Appl Ergon 29:247–254

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baijal S, Srinivasan N (2011) Emotional and hemispheric asymmetries in shifts of attention: an ERP study. Cogn Emot 25:280–294

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Braun CC, Silver NC (1995) Interaction of signal word and colour on warning labels: differences in perceived hazard and behavioural compliance. Ergonomics 38:2207–2220

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chan AH, Ng AW (2009) Perceptions of implied hazard for visual and auditory alerting signals. Saf Sci 47:346–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chica AB, Lupiáñez J (2009) Effects of endogenous and exogenous attention on visual processing: an inhibition of return study. Brain Res 1278:75–85

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen N, Henik A, Moyal N (2012) Executive control attenuates emotional effects—For high reappraisers only? Emotion 12:970

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dai Q, Feng Z (2009) Deficient inhibition of return for emotional faces in depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 33:921–932

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan-Johnson CC, Donchin E (1977) On quantifying surprise: the variation of event-related potentials with subjective probability. Psychophysiology 14:456–467

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis HC, Ashbrook PW (1988) Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states on memory. In: Fiedler K, Forgas J (eds) Affect, cognition and social behavior. Hogrefe, Toronto, pp 25–43

  • Fox E, Russo R, Dutton K (2002) Attentional bias for threat: evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cogn Emot 16:355–379

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hopfinger JB, West VM (2006) Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing. NeuroImage 31:774–789

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein R (1988) Inhibitory tagging system facilitates visual search. Nature 334:430–431

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein RM (2000) Inhibition of return. Trends in cognitive sciences 4:138–147

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kliegel M, Horn AB, Zimmer H (2003) Emotional after-effects on the P3 component of the event-related brain potential. Int J Psychol 38:129–137

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kok A (1990) Internal and external control: a two-factor model of amplitude change of event-related potentials. Acta Psychol 74:213–236

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kok A (2001) On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 38:557–577

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lange W-G, Heuer K, Reinecke A, Becker ES, Rinck M (2008) Inhibition of return is unimpressed by emotional cues. Cogn Emot 22:1433–1456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux JE (1995) Emotion: clues from the brain. Annu Rev Psychol 46:209–235

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luck SJ, Woodman GF, Vogel EK (2000) Event-related potential studies of attention. Trends Cognit Sci 4:432–440

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma Q, Jin J, Wang L (2010) The neural process of hazard perception and evaluation for warning signal words: evidence from event-related potentials. Neurosci Lett 483:206–210

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ma Q, Bian J, Ji W, Tang Q, Xu Q (2011) Research on warnings with new thought of neuro-IE. Procedia Eng 26:1633–1638

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman A, Mineka S (2001) Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol Rev 108:483

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pérez-Dueñas C, Acosta A, Lupiáñez J (2014) Reduced habituation to angry faces: increased attentional capture as to override inhibition of return. Psychol Res 78:196–208

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Polich J (1987) Task difficulty, probability, and inter-stimulus interval as determinants of P300 from auditory stimuli. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol/Evoked Potentials Sect 68:311–320

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Polich J (2007) Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol: Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol 118:2128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Portin R, Kovala T, Polo-Kantola P, Revonsuo A, Müller K, Matikainen E (2000) Does P3 reflect attentional or memory performances, or cognition more generally? Scand J Psychol 41:31–40

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Cohen Y (1984) Components of visual orienting. Atten Perform X: Control Lang Process 32:531–556

    Google Scholar 

  • Pribram KH, McGuinness D (1975) Arousal, activation, and effort in the control of attention. Psychol Rev 82:116

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers WA, Lamson N, Rousseau GK (2000) Warning research: an integrative perspective. Hum Factors: J Hum Factors Ergon Soc 42:102–139

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ryu V, An SK, Jo HH, Cho HS (2010) Decreased P3 amplitudes elicited by negative facial emotion in manic patients: selective deficits in emotional processing. Neurosci Lett 481:92–96

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schupp HT, Flaisch T, Stockburger J, Junghöfer M (2006) Emotion and attention: event-related brain potential studies. Prog Brain Res 156:31–51

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw K, Lien M-C, Ruthruff E, Allen PA (2011) Electrophysiological evidence of emotion perception without central attention. J Cognit Psychol 23:695–708

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoyanova RS, Pratt J, Anderson AK (2007) Inhibition of return to social signals of fear. Emotion 7:49

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tachibana H, Toda K, Sugita M (1992) Age-related changes in attended and unattended P3 latency in normal subjects. Int J Neurosci 66:277

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verkuil B, Brosschot JF, Putman P, Thayer JF (2009) Interacting effects of worry and anxiety on attentional disengagement from threat. Behav Res Ther 47:146–152

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang Y, Wang K, Ma Q (2008) Research on Chinese perceptions of implied hazard for warning signal words and surrounding shapes based on computer experiments. In: Management of Engineering & Technology, 2008. PICMET 2008. Portland International Conference on. IEEE, pp 1628–1633

  • Wogalter M, Silver N (1990) Arousal strength of signal words. Forensic Reports 3

  • Wogalter MS, Kalsher MJ, Frederick LJ, Magurno AB (1998) Hazard level perceptions of warning. Int J Cognit Ergon 2:123–143

    Google Scholar 

  • Wogalter MS, Kalsher MJ, Rashid R (1999) Effect of signal word and source attribution on judgments of warning credibility and compliance likelihood. Int J Ind Ergon 24:185–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wogalter MS, Conzola VC, Smith-Jackson TL (2002) Research-based guidelines for warning design and evaluation. Appl Ergon 33:219–230

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wogalter MS, DeJoy D, Laughery KR (2005) Warnings and risk communication. CRC Press, Boca Raton

    Google Scholar 

  • Yiend J, Mathews A (2001) Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Q J Exp Psychol: Sect A 54:665–681

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yu R, Chan AH, Salvendy G (2004) Chinese perceptions of implied hazard for signal words and surround shapes. Hum Factors Ergon Manuf & Serv Ind 14:69–80

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Philosophy and Social Science Planning Project of Zhejiang Province (15NDJC031YB), Grant No. 71371167 from the National Natural Science Foundation, Project from the Research Center of Information Technology & Economic and Social Development in Zhejiang province (ZX140209304007), and Zhejiang Provincial Key Research base of Humanistic and Social Sciences in Hangzhou Dianzi University (ZD01-201501).

Conflict of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Qingguo Ma.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shang, Q., Huang, Y. & Ma, Q. Hazard levels of warning signal words modulate the inhibition of return effect: evidence from the event-related potential P300. Exp Brain Res 233, 2645–2653 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4335-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4335-4

Keywords

Navigation