Introduction

In behavioral research, triangulation of approaches (i.e., using several tests of the same construct) can be extremely useful for increasing the robustness of the findings, and thus increase the confidence in the validity of the results (Stegenga 2009). In light of this, behavioral test batteries are widely employed, where animals are tested in multiple behavioral tasks either on the same day or across weeks, and the results are triangulated to gain a more robust operational definition of the target behavior (Paylor et al. 2006). While behavioral test batteries are common in rodent research, they have been less widely employed in zebrafish behavioral studies. Instead, there is a tendency to use a larger sample of animals. A drawback of this approach in zebrafish is that it increases the number of potentially unnecessary animals used in research (Born et al. 2017; McIlwain et al. 2001; Tammimäki et al. 2010). However, the systematic assessment of the impact of multiple behavioral tests on zebrafish performance in the assays is yet to be carried out.

Anxiety is a transdiagnostic trait observed across many affective disorders, and understanding more about its underlying biology would assist in the development of novel or repositioned pharmacotherapeutics (Demetriou et al. 2021; Newby et al. 2015). To address this, zebrafish (Danio rerio) have been widely used in the translational neuroscience of affective disorders using anxiety as a core subject of investigation (Cachat et al. 2010; Maximino et al. 2010b; Stewart et al. 2012). The two most commonly employed assays for studying anxiety-like behavior in zebrafish are the novel tank diving task (NTT) and the light–dark test (LDT) (Blaser and Rosemberg 2012). The NTT and LDT have been extensively validated using drugs that induce anxiolytic and anxiogenic effects across species, including humans (Egan et al. 2009; Parker et al. 2012; Rosemberg et al. 2012).

The NTT exploits the natural tendency of zebrafish to dive to the bottom of a novel environment, gradually exploring the top zone of the tank as they habituate to the environment (Levin et al. 2007). In the NTT, anxiety can be operationally defined in terms of (either) time spent in the bottom (↑ time =  = ↑ anxiety), in the top half (↑ time =  = ↓ anxiety), or top third (↑ time =  = ↓ anxiety) (Egan et al. 2009; Gerlai et al. 2000; Parker et al. 2012; Rosemberg et al. 2012) of a novel tank. Similarly, the LDT evaluates the extent of a fishes natural tendency for scototaxis (aversion to bright areas and natural preference for the dark) in a novel environment (Blaser and Penalosa 2011; Facciol et al. 2017). In the LDT, anxiety is operationally defined either by time spent in the light portion (↑ time =  = ↓ anxiety) or more time spent in the dark compartment (↑ time =  = ↑ anxiety) (Blaser and Rosemberg 2012; Facciol et al. 2019; Gerlai et al. 2000; Maximino et al. 2010a; Mezzomo et al. 2016). In both tasks, factors such as lighting, handling, pre-test housing, and the color of the tank play an important role in fish behavioral response (Blaser and Rosemberg 2012; Parker et al. 2012).

Song et al. (2016) evaluated the impact of carrying out the NTT and LDT as a test battery, and found no impact on baseline performance of their animals. In addition, they tested the impact of repeated testing following a mild (bright light) and strong (transportation in a car) stressor. They found no significant differences between the responses of the fish across the two test times, confirming (a) that fish showed very little evidence of a test battery effect, and (b) that this was stable even after a stress challenge. Despite these promising findings, what was not clear from the Song et al.’s (2016) study was whether the test battery effect would be observed following pharmacological manipulations: this is critical to know, as zebrafish are commonly used for psychopharmacology experiments (Cassar et al. 2020; MacRae and Peterson 2015). In addition, and critically, previous studies did not examine individual fish performance across these two behavioral tasks. Because of this, it is not clear if the group effects were also observed in individual animals, or indeed, if individual animals show robust test–retest reliability across the battery.

Here, we had three aims. First, we aimed to evaluate whether testing the same individuals on different anxiety-related tasks (NTT and LDT) would affect either their baseline behavior on the task or the effect size of two well-characterized anxiolytic and anxiogenic interventions (fluoxetine, and conspecific alarm substance; CAS) on their performance. Second, we aimed to examine correlations between NTT and LDT performance endpoints to better understand the value of test batteries for anxiety in zebrafish. Third, we looked at the effects of each drug after a time delay to differentiate if the effect is caused whether by the test battery or time effect.

Material and methods

Animals and experimental design

Zebrafish (AB wild-type) were bred in-house and reared in standard laboratory conditions on a re-circulating system (Aquaneering, USA) on a 14-/10-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 9:00 a.m.), pH 8.4, at ∼28.5 °C (± 1 °C) in groups of 10 animals per 2.8 L. Fish were fed three times/day with a mixture of live brine shrimp and flake food. All behavioral tests were performed between 10:00 and 15:00 h (Mon-Sun).

Figure 1 depicts the experimental design. Adult zebrafish (4 mpf; 50:50 female:male ratio) were first transferred to 300-mL beakers containing either aquarium-treated water for 5 min (handling control), fluoxetine (100 µg/L; 30-min exposure), or conspecific alarm substance (CAS; 5-min exposure), and then transferred either to NTT, LDT, or to new beaker (6 min—time delay groups) (see below). Animals were then immediately transferred to the second anxiety test (NTT or LDT depending on the first task assessed). Animals from the time delay group were tested only in one behavioral task. Fluoxetine was obtained from Sigma-Aldrich (Dorset, UK). After behavioral recording, fish were euthanized using 2-phenoxyethanol from Aqua-Sed (Aqua-Sed™, Vetark, Winchester, UK).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental design illustration showing the behavioral test battery of NTT followed by LDT or vice-versa, and the time delay groups (6 min). For the fluoxetine and conspecific alarm substance (CAS) groups, animals were pretreated a priori behavioral assessment for 30 and 5 min, respectively

Required sample size of ~ 64 for each drug exposure (n = 16 NTT → LDT + n = 16 LDT → NTT + n = 16 time delayed + NTT + n = 16 time delayed + LDT) was calculated a priori following pilot experiments and previous sample size used for testing drug effects in the NTT and LDT in our lab (d = 1.25, power = 0.8, alpha = 0.05). To ensure data reliability, two independent batches were tested (choosing n = 8 fish from several housing tanks each batch). All behavioral testing was carried out in a fully randomized order, randomly choosing fish from one of four housing tanks for drug exposure followed by behavioral testing. After each behavioral trial, the water from the NTT, LDT, and beakers was changed. All experiments were carried out following approval from the University of Portsmouth Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Board, and under license from the UK Home Office (Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986) [PPL: P9D87106F].

Conspecific alarm substance (CAS) extraction

CAS is a fear cue that has been successfully used to trigger stress-related responses at physiological and behavioral levels in different fish species (Abreu et al. 2016; Canzian et al. 2017; Fraker et al. 2009; Hall and Suboski 1995; Quadros et al. 2016; Speedie and Gerlai 2008; Wong et al. 2010). Briefly, CAS exposure was performed by individually exposing fish to 1.05 mL of CAS preparation in 300-mL beakers for 5 min. In order to obtain CAS, a phenotypically similar donor fish was killed using rapid cooling (submersion in 2 °C water). The epidermal cells were then cut with 10 shallow slices on both sides of the body using a razor blade. Ten milliliters of distilled water was then added into a Petri dish and mixed to fully cover the fish’s body. All procedures were performed on ice and controlled to avoid drawing blood and any external contamination (Canzian et al. 2017; Egan et al. 2009; Quadros et al. 2016; Speedie and Gerlai 2008). After CAS exposure, fish were tested in the NTT → LDT or LDT → NTT (counterbalanced 50:50).

Novel tank diving test (NTT)

Animals (n = 144) were placed individually in a purpose-built transparent tank (20 cm length × 17.5 cm height × 5 cm width) containing 1 L of aquarium water. Behavioral activity was analyzed using the Zantiks AD system’s purpose-built NTT (Zantiks Ltd., Cambridge, UK) for 6 min (Egan et al. 2009; Parker et al. 2012; Rosemberg et al. 2012). The Zantiks AD system was fully controlled via a web-enabled device during behavioral training. The tank was separated into three virtual zones (bottom, middle, and top) to provide a detailed evaluation of vertical activity. The following endpoints were analyzed: distance traveled, and time spent in the top zone.

Light–dark test (LDT)

The LDT was performed in a black tank (20 cm length × 15 cm height × 15 cm width) divided into two equally sized partitions where half of the tank area contained a bright white light and the other half was covered with a purpose-built black partition to avoid light exposure. Animals (n = 144) were place individually into the behavioral apparatus and their activity was analyzed using the Zantiks AD system’s purpose-built LDT equipment (Zantiks Ltd., Cambridge, UK) for 6 min to determine the time spent in the dark area (Blaser and Rosemberg 2012; Maximino et al. 2010a; Mezzomo et al. 2016).

Statistics

Normality and homogeneity of variances were ascertained by the Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Bartlett’s test, respectively. Control groups NTT and LDT data were analyzed using one-way ANOVA (baseline behavior NTT/LDT for 1st vs. 2nd vs. delay (6 min)). Two-way ANOVA with multiple testing (two levels: 1st vs. 2nd tested in a new environment or 1st tested vs. time delay for behavioral testing) and substance exposure (between-subjects factor—three levels: control, fluoxetine, CAS) as fixed factors were used to compare anxiety endpoints (NTT, time spent in top of the tank and distance traveled; LDT, time in the dark area). Results were expressed as means ± standard error of the mean (S.E.M). Tukey’s test was used as post-hoc analysis and all the groups were compared between each other. Results were considered significant when p ≤ 0.05. Heat maps were used to summarize differences between groups in the NTT or LDT comparing 1st, 2nd, and time delayed groups. Pearson correlation analysis was used to assess the association between time spent in the top zone and in the lit area for the control, fluoxetine, and CAS groups, independently of testing order.

Results

Multiple testing does not affect anxiety-like behavior in drug-free animals

Figure 2 shows the distance traveled, time spent in top, and time spent in the lit area for control zebrafish tested in both NTT and LDT. For locomotion, no significant effect was observed for the distance traveled in the NTT (F (2, 45) = 0.1485; p = 0.8624). Similarly, no significant difference was observed for controls’ time spent in top (tested 1st vs. 2nd vs. delay (6 min); F (2, 45) = 0.02521; p = 0.9751) (Fig. 2A). Regarding animals’ scototaxis, no significant difference was observed for animals tested in the light–dark test 1st vs. 2nd vs. delay (6 min) (F (2, 45) = 0.2282; p = 0.7969) (Fig. 2B).

Fig. 2
figure 2

The effects of behavioral test battery and time delay in A novel tank diving task and B light–dark test of wild-type (WT) zebrafish. Data were represented as mean ± S.E.M and analyzed by T test (n = 16 per group)

Fluoxetine anxiolytic-like effect is increased with a time delay or when secondly tested

In Fig. 3, the behavioral phenotype of fish exposed to fluoxetine compared to controls tested in the NTT and LDT is compared. There was no significant effect on the distance traveled in terms of test order (F (1,60) = 0.1671; p = 0.6842), fluoxetine (F (1,60) = 0.3467; p = 0.558), or their interaction (F (1,60) = 0.6865; p = 0.4106). There was, however, a significant effect of test order (F (1,60) = 14.00; p*** = 0.0004), fluoxetine (F (1,60) = 37.55; p**** < 0.0001), and interaction between factors (F (1,60) = 12.41; p*** = 0.0008) for time spent in the top zone. A significant increase in the time spent in top for fluoxetine 2nd group compared to controls 1st (p**** < 0.0001), fluoxetine 1st (p**** < 0.0001), and controls 2nd (p**** < 0.0001) was observed (Fig. 3A). Regarding the effects of fluoxetine on multiple testing in the LDT, there was a significant main effect of fluoxetine exposure (F (1,60) = 11.01; p** = 0.0015) but no test order effect (F (1,60) = 2.667; p = 0.1077). No interaction between factors (test order*fluoxetine) was observed for time spent in the lit area (F (1,60) = 1.563; p = 0.2165). Tukey’s post-hoc test showed a significant effect only for fluoxetine 2nd vs. controls 1st (p** = 0.0053) and controls 2nd (p* = 0.0120). No significant effect was observed for fluoxetine 1st vs. fluoxetine 2nd (p = 0.2456) (Fig. 3B).

Fig. 3
figure 3

The effects of behavioral test battery in novel tank diving task (A) and light–dark test (B) of wild-type (WT) zebrafish acutely exposed to fluoxetine 100 µg/L. The effects of time delay in novel tank diving task (C) and light–dark test (D) of WT zebrafish acutely exposed to fluoxetine 100 µg/L. Data were represented as mean ± S.E.M and analyzed by two-way ANOVA (test order and fluoxetine as factors), followed by Tukey’s test multiple comparison test (n = 16 per group)

Similarly, however, no significant effect was found for locomotion in time delay (F (1,60) = 0.8742; p = 0.3535), fluoxetine exposure (F (1,60) = 0.2465; p = 0.6214), and their interaction (F (1,60) = 0.05946; p = 0.8082). A significant effect for time delay (F (1,60) = 4.046; p* = 0.488) and fluoxetine (F (1,60) = 17.92; p**** < 0.0001) was found for time spent in top zone. No significant effect for interaction between factors (time delay*fluoxetine; F (1,60) = 2.977; p = 0.0896) was found for the time spent in top. The fluoxetine time delay group was the only group to significantly increase time spent in the top zone when compared to both control NTT 1st (p*** = 0.0002) and NTT time delay control (p*** = 0.0005) (Fig. 3C). Meanwhile, for the time spent in the lit zone, fluoxetine (F (1,60) = 11.77; p** = 0.0011) and time delay (F (1,60) = 4.345; p* = 0.0011) had a significant effect with no interaction effect being observed (F (1,60) = 1.976; p = 0.1650). However, the only group that had a significant effect compared to both controls (1st LDT and time delay control) was the group fluoxetine time delay (p** = 0.0015 and p** = 0.0068, respectively) (Fig. 3D).

CAS effects are decreased over time in a test battery and after time delay

CAS was used as an anxiogenic control, and its effects on anxiety-like behavior in the NTT test followed by light–dark test and vice-versa are depicted in Fig. 4. A significant interaction effect (test order * CAS exposure) was observed for the distance traveled (F (1,60) = 4.434; p* = 0.0394) but there was no main effect of test order (F (1,60) = 2.312; p = 0.1336) or CAS exposure (F (1,60) = 0.2362; p = 0.6287). However, no significant effect was observed through Tukey’s post-hoc analysis. Regarding animals’ time spent in the top zone, there was a significant main effect of CAS exposure (F (1,60) = 6.326; p* = 0.0146) independent of test order. No significant interaction between factors (F (1,60) = 0.5602; p = 0.4571) or test order (F (1,60) = 1.059; p = 0.3077) effect was observed for CAS exposure. A significant decrease in the time spent in top was only observed for controls 1st vs. CAS 1st after post-hoc analysis (p* = 0.0484) and no significant difference was observed for controls 2nd vs. CAS 2nd (p = 0.3861) (Fig. 4A). Finally, although no interaction (test order vs. CAS exposure; F (1,60) = 0.03571; p = 0.8508) and test order effect (F (1,60) = 0.6211; p = 0.4338) were observed for time spent in the lit zone, a significant CAS exposure effect was observed (F (1,60) = 18.47; p < 0.0001). Briefly, CAS exposure decreased the time spent in the lit area comparing CAS-exposed tested 1st and 2nd with their own controls (p* = 0.0124 and p* = 0.0257, respectively).

Fig. 4
figure 4

The effects of behavioral test battery in novel tank diving task (A) and light–dark test (B) of wild-type (WT) zebrafish acutely exposed to CAS for 5 min. The effects of time delay in novel tank diving task (C) and light–dark test (D) of WT zebrafish acutely exposed to CAS for 5 min. Data were represented as mean ± S.E.M and analyzed by two-way ANOVA (test order and CAS as factors), followed by Tukey’s test multiple comparison test (n = 16 per group)

When looking at the effect of time delay on CAS exposure (Fig. 4C and D), a significant effect was found for CAS only when looking at the time spent in the lit zone (F (1,60) = 11.77; p** = 0.0011) and time spent in the top zone (F (1,60) = 4.351; p* = 0.0413). No significant effects were observed for the interaction between factors (F (1,60) = 0.2666; p = 0.6075) and for time delay (F (1,60) = 2.162; p = 0.1467) for the time spent in the lit zone. Meanwhile, no interaction between factors (F (1,60) = 1.255; p = 0.2727) and time delay effect (F (1,60) = 2.189; p = 0.1443) was observed for time spent in the top zone. Distance traveled was not affected by CAS, time delay, or interaction between factors distance traveled (F (1,60) = 0.9922; p = 0.3232, F (1,60) = 0.1734; p = 0.6786 and F (1,60) = 0.1123; p = 0.7387, respectively). Tukey’s post-hoc test yielded a significant difference only for time spent in the lit zone of 1st LDT + CAS group compared to both 1st LDT (p* = 0.0345) and time delay LDT group (p** = 0.0053). The individual values for the time spent in the top zone and in the lit area for both drugs are summarized in Fig. 5.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Representative heat maps showing the time spent in the top zone and in the lit area for each individual fish from different treatments (control vs. fluoxetine vs. CAS)

Time spent in the top and lit zones is positively correlated

Figure 6 displays intercorrelations between the endpoints for both the behavioral measures, both in the presence of CAS and fluoxetine, and with no-drug treatment. There was a strong positive correlation for the time spent in top (NTT) and time spent in the lit zone (LDT) for the no-drug-treated group (r = 0.6954; p**** < 0.0001; n = 32), and a moderate positive correlation for the fluoxetine group (r = 0.3736; p* = 0.0352; n = 32). However, there was no correlation between endpoints in the tests in the CAS-exposed animals (r = 0.0754; p = 0.6917; n = 32).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Correlational analysis between time spent in the lit area and time spent in the top zone for each group (n = 32)

Discussion

Here, for the first time, we tested how using the same individuals in two anxiety-related tasks affects their behavioral responses to the protocols, both in the absence of drugs and following exposure to an anxiolytic and anxiogenic compound (fluoxetine and CAS, respectively). Additionally, we examined whether introducing a time delay plays a role in the drug response of fluoxetine and CAS, or it is the test battery that increases or decreases animals’ behavioral response in a second task. We also examined, for the first time, how individuals performed across the different tasks to better understand individual performance characteristics in the two tests. We found that parameters linked to anxiety-like behavior, such as the time spent in top and time spent in the lit area, are not affected by testing wild-type (WT) fish in the NTT followed by LDT and vice-versa. However, when animals are exposed to fluoxetine, a larger effect size was observed in the second test, independent of the test (i.e., NTT or LDT) which suggests that there is an impact of the time in which these fish were tested, rather than multiple testing. This hypothesis was later confirmed by testing animals in the NTT or LDT after a time delay with no previous behavioral testing. Differently from fluoxetine, CAS had its higher effect in the when immediately tested; however, similar to what was found for fluoxetine, the results were consistent, independent of the task. Moreover, lower correlation values were found when comparing the time spent in the top zone vs. time spent in the lit area for both fluoxetine- and CAS-exposed animals, suggesting higher data variability when animals are tested in both behavioral tasks and these parameters are compared independently of testing order.

The NTT and the LDT are widely used to assess anxiety-like behavior in zebrafish (Cachat et al. 2010; Maximino et al. 2010b; Stewart et al. 2012). The comparison across tasks has been previously studied, with both tasks demonstrating good cross-test correlation in vivo and similar sensitivity to zebrafish anxiety-like states (Kysil et al. 2017). In rodents, behavioral test battery is commonly used to study several behaviors including anxiety-related tasks using open field and light–dark transitions (Okuda et al. 2018). In zebrafish, studies have used a combination of social behavior, memory, and anxiety tests in order to examine inter-domain correlations, but also to minimize the use of animals (Fontana et al. 2020, 2021). Zebrafish have previously been shown to display a similar behavioral phenotype when tested in the NTT → LDT or LDT → NTT (Song et al. 2016). In the same study, an acute stressor (30-min car transportation) increased anxiety-related patterns in both tasks (Song et al. 2016). These data suggest that a strong effect can still be observed when performing behavioral test batteries in zebrafish, with no impact of test order or multiple testing by itself. Here, we found that the overall baseline response of our control WT zebrafish is kept the same across tasks when testing NTT and LDT, which supports the use of this species across anxiety-related tasks in order to reduce the number of animals used in research. However, when animals were acutely exposed to CAS or fluoxetine, different effects were observed when performing a behavioral test battery.

A significant role for fluoxetine in decreasing anxiety is more pronounced in the 2nd behavioral task or after 6 min of time delay, independently of it being NTT or LDT. No significant differences were observed in the 1st behavioral task. Fluoxetine is a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) commonly used to treat several psychiatric disorders in humans. The role of fluoxetine in zebrafish anxiety is somewhat controversial. For example, Stewart et al. (2011b) showed that fluoxetine had no effect on anxiety-like behavior at a concentration of 0.1 mg/L (the same concentration used here). However, these animals had an increased tendency to spend time in the top zone of the tank, which can be an indicator of decreased anxiety. The authors discussed that the lack of anxiolytic effects following acute fluoxetine in zebrafish contradicts clinical and rodent findings (Hascoët et al. 2000; Lightowler et al. 1994; Varty et al. 2002). Here, the anxiolytic effect of fluoxetine was only observed when fish were tested in the second task or after 6 min. This could explain the data variability across papers, since we observed no effect when fish were immediately tested in the NTT or LDT. Altogether, these data suggest that there is a temporal delay in the effects of fluoxetine. The pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of fluoxetine vary depending on administration route and time/duration of its exposure (Caccia et al. 1990; Sawyer and Howell 2011). For example, a study with non-human primates showed that the peak of fluoxetine in serum is achieved at different times depending on the drug concentration (15 min for 31 ng/mL, 30 min for 70 ng/mL, and 60 min for 165 ng/mL). Meanwhile, its main metabolite, norfluoxetine, was only found at 120 min for all the doses tested lasting up to 24 h after fluoxetine exposure (Sawyer and Howell 2011). Although fluoxetine has been commonly used as an anxiolytic drug in neuropsychiatric studies using zebrafish, time-dose–response studies looking at the effects of fluoxetine using water exposure as the administration route in this species is lacking. A recent study has explored the long-term effects of fluoxetine in zebrafish behavior up to 28 days after acute exposure to different concentrations, where the authors found that the fluoxetine effects vary depending on time of behavioral testing (Al Shuraiqi et al. 2021). However, fluoxetine short time-dose–response in anxiety-related paradigms is still unknown being an important step for the understanding of data variability across labs and the mechanisms underlying fluoxetine inducing anxiolytic and anxiogenic phenotypes.

Although a significant decrease in the time spent in the lit area could be observed when animals were tested 1st, 2nd, or after time delay in the LDT, in the NTT, no significant effects were observed between controls and CAS-exposed animals when tested 2nd or after time delay. CAS is an effective acute stressor which is produced and stored in the epidermal “club” cells and is naturally released into the water after skin injuries provoked by predator bouts (Chivers and Smith 1994; Korpi and Wisenden 2001). The different concentrations and effects of CAS on zebrafish fear and anxiety-related behavior were first described by Speedie and Gerlai (2008) being its anxiogenic effects well-characterized in behavioral neuroscience research. For example, in the light–dark test, zebrafish exposed to CAS for 5 min showed increased scototaxis (preference for dark areas) (Abreu et al. 2016; Quadros et al. 2016) which is a behavioral change often observed after the exposure to anxiogenic drugs (Stewart et al. 2011a). Similarly, we found that CAS significantly decreased the time spent in the lit area and in the top zone, which indicates an increased “anxious” response. Interestingly, we found that this effect is attenuated in the second task only when NTT is the second behavioral analysis in the test battery, where a strong effect is maintained across tasks for the LDT. Similarly, when considering time delay as a factor, no significant differences were observed for CAS in the NTT, but a strong effect was still observed in the LDT even after a time delay. However, in both behavioral tasks, the effect size of CAS-induced anxiogenic behavior is decreased when animals are tested after 6 min suggesting that these effects could potentially decrease across time.

When looking at the correlation between these tasks, independent of the test order, there was a strong positive correlation between the time spent in the top zone and time spent in the lit area, making the behavioral phenotypes in those tasks comparable. Similarly, a previous study has showed that those tasks show good cross-test correlation with NTT only differing from LDT in terms of cortisol responses after tasks where NTT is correlated to higher stress-related responses (Kysil et al. 2017). In addition, here, we showed that the correlation between NTT and LDT anxiety-related variables is not always good showing low values when animals are exposed to different molecules such as fluoxetine and CAS. Although this data could indicate that data is less reliable when comparing the animals’ response in both tasks (NTT → LDT or LDT → NTT), the main effects for these drugs when tested firstly (CAS) and secondly (fluoxetine) were similar across behavioral tasks.

Conclusion

Overall, the use of behavioral battery testing for anxiety-like behavior can indeed influence behavioral response when fish are previously exposed to a chemical substance, such as CAS or fluoxetine. However, our data indicate that the effects are not caused by the test battery per se but rather by the test time. For example, fluoxetine has higher anxiolytic-like effects when tested secondly or after a time delay. Meanwhile, CAS effects are higher in the first behavior task compared to the second behavioral task or after 6 min. Importantly, WT behavior was not influenced by testing animals in both new environments. Our findings may be particularly important for characterization of mutant lines, where a reduced number of animals could potentially be used to evaluate baseline behavior when there is no influence of drug exposure. However, further studies are still necessary to compare data between WT animals and genetically altered fish. Altogether, this supports the use of baseline behavior assessment using multiple tasks; however, researchers must carefully prepare their experimental design when testing drugs and conducting behavioral battery considering the drug time-dose–response.