Skip to main content
Log in

Associations among exposure to television or video, language development, and school achievement in childhood: a prospective birth cohort study

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Purpose

Exposure to electronic media, which begins in early childhood, has been gradually increasing. This study investigated the associations between time watching television or video (TV time), children’s language development, and school achievement using a large prospective design.

Methods

The participants underwent annual assessment from birth (T1; 5.5 months) to T8 (87.9 months). We used a parental questionnaire to evaluate TV time annually from T3 (2.2 years) to T8 (7.3 years), by the item asking “How many hours does your child usually spend a day watching TV or videos?”. Children were grouped by TV time using group-based trajectory analysis. Among 2150 participants, 1087 were included in the final analysis. The Korean version of the Denver II and the Korean-Ages and Stages questionnaires were administered to evaluate early childhood development (T1 to T3). The Receptive and Expressive Vocabulary Test was used to assess children’s language development at T8. Questionnaires completed by both parents and teachers were used to evaluate school achievement at T8.

Results

Participants were divided into “Low Descending” (LD), “Medium Flat” (MF), “High Ascending” (HA), and “Extremely High Descending” (XHD) groups. Early language development at T1 and T2 did not significantly differ among all groups. However, language development at T3 and T8, and school achievement at T8 differed significantly, and were the highest in LD and lowest in HA.

Conclusion

We found a negative association between greater TV time and children’s language and cognitive development. Thus, unchecked TV exposure could be detrimental to childhood cognitive development.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Abbreviations

T1:

Time 1

T2:

Time 2

T3:

Time 3

T4:

Time 4

T5:

Time 5

T6:

Time 6

T7:

Time 7

T8:

Time 8

LD:

Low descending

MF:

Medium flat

HA:

High ascending

XHD:

Extremely high descending

REVT:

Receptive and expressive vocabulary test

K-ASQ:

Korean-ages and stage questionnaires

References

  1. Council on Communications and Media, Brown A (2011) Media use by children younger than 2 years. Pediatrics 128(5):1040–1045. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-1753

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S (2019) Association between screen time and children’s performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr 173(3):244–250. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5056

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN (2007) Television and DVD/video viewing in children younger than 2 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 161(5):473–479. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.161.5.473

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Rideout V (2017) The common sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight. Common Sense Media, San Francisco, pp 263–283

    Google Scholar 

  5. Chonchaiya W, Pruksananonda C (2008) Television viewing associates with delayed language development. Acta Paediatr 97(7):977–982. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00831.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Berkule S, Fierman AH, Brockmeyer C, Mendelsohn AL (2010) Infant media exposure and toddler development. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 164(12):1105–1111. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.235

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Duch H, Fisher EM, Ensari I, Font M, Harrington A, Taromino C, Yip J, Rodriguez C (2013) Association of screen time use and language development in Hispanic toddlers: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Clin Pediatr 52(9):857–865. https://doi.org/10.1177/0009922813492881

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Nathanson AI, Aladé F, Sharp ML, Rasmussen EE, Christy K (2014) The relation between television exposure and executive function among preschoolers. Dev Psychol 50(5):1497–1506. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035714

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA (2005) Children’s television viewing and cognitive outcomes: a longitudinal analysis of national data. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 159(7):619–625

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Fisch SM, Truglio RT (2014) G is for growing: thirty years of research on children and Sesame Street. Routledge, UK

    Book  Google Scholar 

  11. Radesky JS, Schumacher J, Zuckerman B (2015) Mobile and interactive media use by young children: the good, the bad, and the unknown. Pediatrics 135(1):1–3. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-2251

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Kirkorian HL, Choi K, Pempek TA (2016) Toddlers’ word learning from contingent and noncontingent video on touch screens. Child Dev 87(2):405–413. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12508

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Carson V, Kuzik N, Hunter S, Wiebe SA, Spence JC, Friedman A, Tremblay MS, Slater LG, Hinkley T (2015) Systematic review of sedentary behavior and cognitive development in early childhood. Prev Med 78:115–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.07.016

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Sameroff A (2010) A unified theory of development: a dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Dev 81(1):6–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01378.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Nisbett R (2004) The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently… and why. Simon and Schuster, New York

    Google Scholar 

  16. Nisbett RE, Miyamoto Y (2005) The influence of culture: holistic versus analytic perception. Trends Cogn Sci 9(10):467–473

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hoff E (2006) How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev Rev 26(1):55–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Lee KS et al (2005) The effect of extreme exposure to mass media on young children's development. Korean J Dev Psychol 18(2):75–103

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Byeon H, Hong S (2015) Relationship between television viewing and language delay in toddlers: evidence from a Korea national cross-sectional survey. PLoS ONE 10(3):e0120663. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120663

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Frankenburg WK, Dodds J, Archer P, Shapiro H, Bresnick B (1992) The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver Developmental Screening Test. Pediatrics 89(1):91–97

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Shin H, Kwon B, Lim S (2005) Validity of Korean version of Denver II in screening children with developmental risk. Korean J Child Health Nurs 11(3):316–321

    Google Scholar 

  22. Squires J, Bricker D, Potter L (1997) Revision of a parent-completed developmental screening tool: ages and stages questionnaires. J Pediatr Psychol 22(3):313–328. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/22.3.313

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Heo KH, Squires J, Lee SY, Lee, JS (2006) KASQ: Korean Ages and Stages Questionnaires/parent-completed development screening tool.

  24. Kim YT, Hong GH, Kim KH (2009) Content and reliability analyses of the receptive and expressive vocabulary test (REVT). Commun Sci Disord 14(1):34–45

    Google Scholar 

  25. Nagin DS, Odgers CL (2010) Group-based trajectory modeling in clinical research. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 6:109–138. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131413

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Little RJA (1988) A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J Am Stat Assoc 83(404):1198–1202. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Bittman M, Rutherford L, Brown J, Unsworth L (2012) Digital natives? New and old media and children’s language acquisition. Fam Matters 91:18–26

    Google Scholar 

  28. Schmidt ME, Rich M, Rifas-Shiman SL, Oken E, Taveras EM (2009) Television viewing in infancy and child cognition at 3 years of age in a US cohort. Pediatrics 123(3):e370–e375. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-3221

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Christakis DA, Gilkerson J, Richards JA, Zimmerman FJ, Garrison MM, Xu D, Gray S, Yapanel U (2009) Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 163(6):554–558. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.61

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN (2007) Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. J Pediatr 151(4):364–368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.04.071

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Department of Health (2010) Move and play every day: National physical activity recommendations for children 0–5 years. Commonwealth of Australia.

  32. Tremblay MS, Leblanc AG, Carson V et al (2012) Canadian sedentary behaviour guidelines for the early years (aged 0–4 years). Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 37(2):370–391. https://doi.org/10.1139/h2012-019

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Scarborough HS (1989) Prediction of reading disability from familial and individual differences. J Educ Psychol 81(1):101–108. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.81.1.101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Pagani LS, Fitzpatrick C, Barnett TA, Dubow E (2010) Prospective associations between early childhood television exposure and academic, psychosocial, and physical well-being by middle childhood. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 164(5):425–431. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.50

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Pagani LS, Fitzpatrick C, Barnett TA (2013) Early childhood television viewing and kindergarten entry readiness. Pediatr Res 74(3):350–355. https://doi.org/10.1038/pr.2013.105

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Ferguson CJ, Donnellan MB (2014) Is the association between children’s baby video viewing and poor language development robust? A reanalysis of Zimmerman, Christakis, and Meltzoff (2007). Dev Psychol 50(1):129–137. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033628

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Rideout VJ, Hamel E (2006) The media family: electronic media in the lives of infants, toddlers, preschoolers and their parents. Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation, California

    Google Scholar 

  38. Lagercrantz H (2016) Connecting the brain of the child from synapses to screen-based activity. Acta Paediatr 105(4):352–357. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.13298

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Domingues-Montanari S (2017) Clinical and psychological effects of excessive screen time on children. J Paediatr Child Health 53(4):333–338. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpc.13462

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Rice ML, Huston AC, Truglio R, Wright JC (1990) Words from “Sesame Street”: learning vocabulary while viewing. Dev Psychol 26(3):421–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.3.421

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Nathanson AI, Sharp ML, Aladé F, Rasmussen EE, Christy K (2013) The relation between television exposure and theory of mind among preschoolers. J Commun 63(6):1088–1108. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12062

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Fidler AE, Zack E, Barr R (2010) Television viewing patterns in 6-to 18-month-olds: the role of caregiver–infant interactional quality. Infancy 15(2):176–196. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2009.00013.x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Richert RA, Robb MB, Fender JG, Wartella E (2010) Word learning from baby videos. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 164(5):432–437. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.24

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank the Korea Institute of Child Care and Education, which provided the raw data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Prof. Kim and Prof. Chung conceptualized and designed the study, drafted the initial manuscript, and reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Un Sun Chung.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

Ethics approval

This study was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Korean Institute of Child Care of Education.

Consent to participate

We provided the parents of participating children with information on the purpose and procedure of the study, and all gave their written informed consent before participating.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 25 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, K.M., Chung, U.S. Associations among exposure to television or video, language development, and school achievement in childhood: a prospective birth cohort study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 56, 847–856 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-020-01967-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-020-01967-w

Keywords

Navigation