figure a

Introduction

Hypoglycaemia is the major limiting factor for intensive glycaemic control in both type 1 and type 2 diabetes mellitus [1, 2]. While hypoglycaemia differs in severity, it remains potentially harmful, impacting cognitive, metabolic, cardiac and neural processes [3]. Hypoglycaemia may also have a significant negative impact on quality of life [4, 5].

Cognitive function refers to all aspects of thinking and intellectual activity, and cognitive processes are fundamental for performing many everyday tasks. Even subtle changes in cognitive function can potentially influence a person’s capacity to function optimally and cognitive impairment raises safety concerns for many occupational activities. In individuals with type 1 diabetes and healthy control groups, acute insulin-induced hypoglycaemia (i.e. plasma glucose levels of 2.2–3.1 mmol/l) has been widely studied and shown to impair performance across several cognitive tests (measuring working memory, attention and reaction time, among others) [6,7,8,9,10]. Acute cognitive decline during hypoglycaemia in individuals with type 1 diabetes can thus impair their ability to tackle cognitive challenges both in daily life and in occupational settings. Nevertheless, it is less well established whether individuals with type 2 diabetes will experience similar cognitive impairment from acute hypoglycaemia due to the underlying pathophysiological differences between type 1 and type 2 diabetes, such as the presence of insulin resistance, which may impact the brain negatively in type 2 diabetes [11]. Furthermore, methodological problems may limit comparisons between studies [12].

The effect of hypoglycaemia on cognitive function in type 2 diabetes above the level considered clinically important by the International Hypoglycaemia Study Group [3] is still unclear. The few reports on cognitive function at higher glucose levels (≥3.0 mmol/l) have focused on determining counter-regulatory thresholds for symptomatic and hormonal responses of hypoglycaemia [13,14,15,16]. In these study designs, cognitive testing has been secondary and, although cognitive impairment has been demonstrated, the number of neurocognitive tests performed has been few, with no assessment of global cognitive function. Therefore, at glucose levels of 3.0 mmol/l and above, further clarification is needed as to whether cognitive function is affected in the type 2 diabetes population, and, if so, which cognitive domains are affected.

Here, we examined cognitive function during an acute, non-severe hypoglycaemic episode (target plasma glucose 3 mmol/l) in participants with type 2 diabetes using an extensive neurocognitive test battery. By averaging scores from the examined cognitive domains, a global cognition score was constructed estimating global cognition. A measurement for psychomotor speed (Symbol Digit Modalities Test [SDMT]) was selected as the primary outcome, as it was previously proven to be sensitive to hypoglycaemia [9, 17]. We hypothesised that non-severe insulin-induced hypoglycaemia would reduce processing speed (primary hypothesis) and global cognition (secondary hypothesis). Counter-regulatory hormones and C-peptide were determined for exploratory analyses.

Methods

Study population

Individuals diagnosed with type 2 diabetes for at least 3 months were approached for participation. They were eligible if their diabetes was treated with diet or glucose-lowering medications (except sulfonylureas or insulin), age was 35–70 years, BMI was 23–35 kg/m2 and HbA1c was below 75 mmol/mol (9%). Key exclusion criteria included recent ischaemic cardiac disease, severe diabetic complications or other major illnesses. Full inclusion and exclusion criteria are listed in the electronic supplementary material (ESM) Table 1. Randomisation was determined by sequential enrolment and lowest available number assignment. Participants were blinded to the sequence of glycaemic conditions and to their current blood glucose concentration.

All participants gave oral and written consent before participation. The study was designed and performed in accordance to the Helsinki Declaration of Good Clinical Practice and was approved by the local ethics committee. The study was registered at www.clinicaltrials.gov (ClinicalTrials.gov registration no. NCT03014011).

Study design

Cognitive performance was measured during hypoglycaemic and euglycaemic conditions in a randomised, double-blinded, crossover study design (ESM Fig. 1). Hypoglycaemic (aiming for a plasma glucose target of 3.0 ± 0.2 mmol/l) and euglycaemic (plasma glucose clamp target 6.0 ± 0.2 mmol/l) clamps were performed on separate visits. The two experimental visits were separated by 21–42 days to avoid effects of counter-regulatory hormone responses or other physiological effects of hypoglycaemia. Blood samples were drawn at each visit for determination of insulin, C-peptide, adrenaline (epinephrine), noradrenaline (norepinephrine), cortisol, growth hormone and glucagon; these were taken at arrival (baseline), 10 min prior to neurocognitive testing and after testing when glucose levels were normalised.

Experimental visit procedure

Participants were instructed to maintain a regular diet and avoid alcohol and strenuous physical exercise 3 days prior to each experimental day to avoid extreme fluctuations in blood glucose levels. After an overnight fast, a cannula was inserted in an antecubital vein for blood sampling. The arm was kept heated with a heating pad throughout the examination to obtain vasodilation as a proxy for arterialisation. A second cannula was inserted into an antecubital vein in the contralateral arm for continuous infusion of glucose (20%) and human soluble insulin (Actrapid, Novo Nordisk, Bagsværd, Denmark). After a 15 min baseline period, a 3 min priming regimen commenced. Then, insulin was infused at a constant rate of 100 mU m−2 min−1 and this continued throughout neurocognitive testing. A variable infusion of glucose was given simultaneously and adjusted to maintain target glucose levels. Blood sampling for glucose was made at 5 min intervals throughout the clamp procedure. Plasma glucose was measured by the glucose oxidase method, using a glucose analyser (Yellow Springs Instrument, Yellow Springs, OH, USA). Neurocognitive testing was initiated when glucose levels had been stable for 40 min. When testing was completed, the participants were served a meal and glucose levels were monitored until euglycaemia was safely maintained (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental visit design. Blood sampling times for noradrenaline, adrenaline, insulin, C-peptide, glucagon, cortisol and growth hormone are shown; blood was also sampled every 5 min for plasma glucose. PG, plasma glucose

Neurocognitive test battery

The test battery included the following neurocognitive tests: the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) [18, 19] (verbal memory), the Trail Making Test (TMT) parts A and B [20] (psychomotor speed and set shifting/task switching [a measure of executive function]), the SDMT [21] (psychomotor speed), the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III (WAIS-III) Letter–Number Sequencing test [22] (working memory), the verbal fluency test (letters S and D [23]; one aspect of executive function) and the Rapid Visual Processing (RVP) test from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) (sustained attention). To minimise learning effects between the two visits, alternate versions of the tests that are associated with greatest learning effects were used. The total duration of neurocognitive testing in each examination was approximately 45 min.

During data analysis, four cognitive domains were constructed based on the neurocognitive tests. Global cognition was defined by the averaging of these domains into a global cognitive score [24].

As the primary outcome test assessing psychomotor speed we chose the SDMT (WPS; www.wpspublish.com). This test consists of a key of nine geometric elements, each associated with a number from 1 to 9. The participant’s score is the number of correct substitutions made within an interval of 120 s.

Neurocognitive testing was conducted by research assistants from the Neurocognition and Emotion in Affective Disorders (NEAD) Group, Psychiatric Centre Copenhagen (www.neadgroup.org), who were blinded to the participants’ glucose concentrations.

Hormones

Counter-regulatory hormones were measured to ensure that the plasma glucose target was sufficiently low to elicit a counter-regulatory response. Plasma noradrenaline and adrenaline were analysed by LC-MS/MS. Plasma samples were extracted by solid phase extraction and the eluate was evaporated and reconstituted before analysis by LC-MS/MS. The analyses were calibrated by in-house prepared calibrators and the relative SD was below 10%. Glucagon was measured by a commercially available ELISA kit (Mercodia, Uppsala, Sweden) according to the manufacturer’s procedure. Three controls at various levels (Mercodia) were included in each assay. The relative SD of the assay controls was below 7% and in the range of the stated values. Insulin, C-peptide, growth hormone and cortisol were determined using the IMMULITE 2000 immunoassay system (Siemens Healthcare, Erlangen, Germany). All samples for a particular hormone were assayed in the same batch.

Symptoms of hypoglycaemia

On each experimental visit, participants were informed of the characteristic symptoms of hypoglycaemia (as stated in the Edinburgh Hypoglycaemia Scale [25]) and instructed to inform the investigator of any symptom occurring during the clamp procedure.

Statistical analyses

Based on the above neurocognitive test battery, four cognitive domains were constructed: verbal memory (consisting of RAVLT total recall across five trials, immediate recall following interference, delayed recall and recognition), executive function and working memory (consisting of TMT-B, WAIS-III letter-number sequencing, verbal fluency), sustained attention (consisting of RVP A′, RVP mean latency) and psychomotor speed (consisting of SDMT and TMT-A). The domains were constructed by averaging standardised test scores for individual tests included in the domain. Standardisation was achieved using residuals from separate linear mixed-effect models with visit as fixed effect and participant as random effect, thereby minimising the influence of potential minor learning effects present during the second experimental visit. A global cognitive score was constructed by averaging the four domain scores [24].

For each glycaemic condition, the neurocognitive test scores were compared using linear mixed-effect models with glycaemic condition and visit interaction as fixed effects and participant as a random effect. Mean differences in cognitive function between hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia were estimated from the models and corresponding 95% CI and p values were calculated. Scores where larger values represent worse performance were inversed in the analyses of cognitive domains and global cognition. Bonferroni correction for multiple testing was applied, providing a level of significance of p = 0.0031.

Effect sizes from individual tests were estimated from Cohen’s d, calculated by the difference between the two glycaemic conditions divided by the residual SD.

Univariable linear regression analyses were performed to evaluate associations between changes in cognitive function (primary outcome, cognitive domains and global cognition) and the following variables: age, HbA1c, counter-regulatory hormones and fasting C-peptide. The difference between sexes and hypoglycaemic symptom severity rating (none/mild vs moderate/severe) on change in cognitive function was analysed by two-sampled t test. Further, logistic regression analyses were used to identify potential associations between hypoglycaemic symptom severity and the variables HbA1c, adrenaline, noradrenaline, growth hormone and cortisol. Model assumptions were validated by visual inspection of residuals and normal probability plots.

The primary endpoint of the study was the SDMT. In a previous trial [26], the SD for Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST, similar to SDMT) between hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia was 9. We assumed a similar level of variance and opted to detect differences of 6 or more. A two-sided paired t test and 5% significance level suggested a sample size of 25 participants completing both experimental visits to achieve a 90% power to detect a true difference in cognitive function. A total of 28 participants were randomised to ensure that at least 25 participants completed the study. Graphical illustrations were performed using GraphPad Prism (version 7.02; GraphPad software; https://www.graphpad.com/scientific-software/prism/) and statistical analyses were performed by R (version 3.2.3; http://cran.r-project.org/).

Results

Participants

We screened 37 participants between May 2017 and July 2018, of whom 28 met inclusion criteria and were randomised, and 25 completed both experimental visits (ESM Fig. 2). Age was a mean ± SD of 60 ± 7.3 years, BMI 30.5 ± 3.4 kg/m2 and HbA1c 53.6 ± 9.4 mmol/mol (7.1 ± 0.9%). The participants’ diabetes duration at inclusion was a mean ± SD of 6.2 ± 3.7 years; data are presented for completers only (Table 1). Three participants (one man, two women) withdrew from the study after randomisation for reasons not related to the intervention (difficulties with venous catheter [one], difficulties with fasting after completion of the first examination day [one] and withdrawal of consent prior to the first day of examination [one]). A stable glucose plateau was achieved during each condition and the individual glucose profiles are displayed in Fig. 2. Mean plasma glucose concentration (± SD) during the neurocognitive testing was 3.13 (± 0.27) mmol/l for the hypoglycaemic clamp and 5.83 (± 0.31) mmol/l for the euglycaemic clamp.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of completers
Fig. 2
figure 2

Plasma glucose concentrations during (a) hypoglycaemia and (b) euglycaemia. The figures show glucose profiles during the first 50 min of the clamp procedure, during the 40 min period of stable glucose concentrations before cognitive testing and during cognitive testing. The x-axis break represents time to establish stable glucose levels, which varied between participants. The solid blue line represents mean glucose concentration; dashed lines represent the individual glucose profiles

Cognitive function

For the primary outcome (SDMT), we found a statistically significant difference in performance with mean ± SD SDMT scores of 48.7 ± 9.8 (hypoglycaemia) and 56.6 ± 12.0 (euglycaemia). The estimated treatment difference (ETD) was −7.9 (95% CI −10.9, −4.9; p < 0.0001; Cohen’s d = 1.01) (Table 2, Fig. 3a, b). Furthermore, global cognitive score showed a highly significant deterioration during the hypoglycaemic condition of −0.7 (95% CI −0.9, −0.6; p < 0.0001) (Table 2, Fig. 3c, d). Finally, analysis of the individual cognitive domains revealed that they were all substantially affected by hypoglycaemia, with varying effect sizes (verbal memory ETD −0.8 [95% CI −1.1, −0.4], p < 0.0001; executive function ETD −0.7 [95% CI −1.0, −0.4], p < 0.0001; psychomotor speed ETD −0.8 [95% CI −1.1, −0.5], p < 0.0001; and sustained attention ETD −0.7 [95% CI −1.1, −0.3], p = 0.0003). The most substantial effect sizes (Cohen’s d > 0.8) were within the primary outcome, letter-number sequencing and mean latency, with performances deteriorating by 14%, 13% and 20%, respectively.

Table 2 Cognitive assessment during hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia
Fig. 3
figure 3

Results of (a, b) SDMT and (c, d) global cognitive score during hypoglycaemic and euglycaemic conditions. (a, b) Red represents participants where the number of correct answers during SDMT was lower during hypoglycaemia than euglycaemia. Blue represents participants where the number of correct answers during SDMT was lower during euglycaemia than hypoglycaemia. (c, d) Red represents participants where the global cognitive score was lower during hypoglycaemia than euglycaemia. Data from one participant are missing owing to technical difficulties

Regression analyses revealed that poorer glycaemic control, indicated by increased HbA1c, was negatively associated with change in global cognitive score during hypoglycaemia (β −0.02; 95% CI −0.04, −0.00; p = 0.042). Glycaemic control did not associate with other examined cognitive outcomes (i.e. primary outcome and cognitive domains) (p > 0.05). Further, no association was found between age, fasting C-peptide or counter-regulatory hormones and change in cognitive function (p > 0.05), and no difference in change of cognitive function was found between sexes (p > 0.05).

Participants with moderate/severe symptom severity had a greater decrease in verbal memory during hypoglycaemia compared with participants with symptom severity rated as none/mild (mean difference of −1.00; 95% CI −1.64, −0.37; p = 0.003). No other cognitive domain, the primary outcome or global cognitive score differed between the two symptom severity ratings (p > 0.05). In addition, we examined a possible association between baseline HbA1c and hypoglycaemic symptomatic severity, showing that odds of moderate/severe symptom severity during hypoglycaemia were increased by 13% (OR 1.13; 95% CI 1.02, 1.29; p = 0.035) for each increase in HbA1c. Similar results were observed for adrenaline and noradrenaline, with the odds of moderate/severe hypoglycaemic symptom severity increasing with adrenaline (OR 1.11; 95% CI 1.04, 1.25; p = 0.021) and noradrenaline (OR 9.50; 95% CI 1.60, 147.31; p = 0.043) responses. No association with symptom severity was found for cortisol or growth hormone (p > 0.05).

Hormonal responses

Cortisol and growth hormone levels were increased during hypoglycaemia compared with euglycaemia and this increase remained when euglycaemia was established after neurocognitive testing in the hypoglycaemic condition. The plasma levels of noradrenaline, adrenaline and glucagon also increased during hypoglycaemia compared with euglycaemia. C-peptide response was diminished during hypoglycaemia. There was no change in insulin levels between the two glycaemic conditions (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Counter-regulatory hormones during hypoglycaemia (black circles) and euglycaemia (white circles) at baseline, clamp (stable hypoglycaemia or euglycaemia) and recovery (after neurocognitive testing and at normalised blood glucose levels). (a) Insulin, (b) C-peptide, (c) cortisol, (d) growth hormone, (e) adrenaline, (f) noradrenaline and (g) glucagon. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05 and ***p < 0.001 for hypoglycaemia vs euglycaemia

Symptoms and side effects

Twenty of the 25 participants (80%) reported symptoms related to hypoglycaemia (ESM Table 2). No examination was discontinued due to the reported hypoglycaemic symptoms. Based on symptoms reported by the participants and an assessment by the investigator, the severity of hypoglycaemic symptoms was categorised as ‘none’, ‘mild’, ‘moderate’ or ‘severe’. One participant reported headache on the euglycaemic control day. Otherwise, there were no reported side effects on the control days.

Discussion

In this study, we demonstrate a substantial cognitive deterioration in participants with type 2 diabetes during hypoglycaemia compared with euglycaemia, with mean plasma glucose values of 3.1 mmol/l and 5.8 mmol/l, respectively. The observed deterioration was highly significant for psychomotor speed (primary outcome) as well as global cognition (secondary outcome), and across all examined cognitive domains (verbal memory, executive function, sustained attention). Notably, a glucose level of 3.1 mmol/l is higher than the majority of previous studies assessing cognitive function during hypoglycaemia and slightly above the glycaemic threshold for impairment of cognitive function stated by the International Hypoglycaemia Study Group in 2017 [3]. Still, we demonstrate a significant and uniform impact of non-severe hypoglycaemia on cognitive function, and also after Bonferroni correction for multiple testing. This impact on cognitive function does not associate with age, fasting C-peptide or counter-regulatory hormones nor does it differ between sexes. This suggests that even non-severe hypoglycaemia in type 2 diabetes may impact on an individual’s daily life, for instance by causing a deterioration in executive functions and a reduced ability to plan, organise and make decisions.

The estimated effect sizes (Cohen’s d) for the cognitive domains demonstrated in this study are all equal to or above 0.7, suggesting a moderate to high effect size. It is known that diabetes can lead to cognitive impairment: meta-analyses demonstrate cognitive deterioration in individuals with type 2 diabetes compared with healthy controls, with effects sizes ranging from −0.26 to −0.36 in several cognitive domains (motor function executive function, processing speed, verbal memory and visual memory) [27]. There is no consensus regarding a clinically relevant treatment effect size, but a change of 0.2–0.4 (small/moderate effect size) has been suggested [24]. We see the most substantial effect sizes within the domains of executive function, psychomotor speed and sustained attention, with the latter demonstrating an approximately 20% deterioration in reaction time. This delay in reaction time might be an important factor in many occupational activities, including the ability to drive. Impairment within the psychomotor speed domain has been identified as a predictor for driving assessment outcome, with lower SDMT scores seen in individuals that failed an on-road assessment [28].

Since insulin may also affect cognitive function [29, 30], we aimed to obtain similar plasma levels of insulin during the two conditions of neurocognitive testing. This goal was achieved. As expected, there was a significant difference in C-peptide levels, indicating that endogenous insulin levels were suppressed during hypoglycaemia. The counter-regulatory hormone levels confirmed that a glucose level of 3.1 mmol/l is sufficient to induce a significant stress response, with increases in growth hormone, cortisol, noradrenaline and adrenaline.

This study shows that non-severe hypoglycaemia affects a broad range of cognitive functions: both simple functions, such as reaction time, and more complex functions such as memory and executive function. However, our design does not allow for a definition of glycaemic threshold for individual cognitive tests or overall cognitive impairment in type 2 diabetes, as it includes a single-step hypoglycaemic plateau only [31]. Our focus was to clearly establish whether cognitive impairment was present at a specific non-severe hypoglycaemic glucose level and, if so, which domains were affected. In addition, the population examined does not necessarily represent individuals that are at risk of hypoglycaemia, i.e. fairly good glycaemic control and no glucose-lowering treatment that could cause hypoglycaemia. Indeed, we chose to exclude participants treated with sulfonylureas and insulin to avoid potential episodes of hypoglycaemia in the days prior to examination, but we expect that drug-induced hypoglycaemia would cause similar cognitive impairments. Furthermore, blood samples were collected from an antecubital vein, with the arm kept heated to obtain vasodilation, as a proxy for arterialisation. Ideally, arterial blood concentrations should be measured, but arterial cannulation is associated with difficulties and risks. This site of collection was chosen to ensure free movement of the hands for neurocognitive testing. Based on previous findings [32], we expect that our glucose values correspond well with glucose values obtained from arterialised venous blood from the hand. If blood glucose levels were underestimated by this procedure, the study would even further support our findings on cognitive function during hypoglycaemia. Finally, we did not test cognitive function during the recovery phase from hypoglycaemia, which has been examined in similar studies [9, 33, 34]. Findings of delayed recovery are not universal, and different cognitive tests seem to give different recovery times [35]. Here, we find that the demonstrated cognitive dysfunction during hypoglycaemia was substantial, and thus a potential delayed recovery phase should be considered for an individual’s daily life.

Currently, there is no consensus suggesting a single test for measurement of cognitive function. The neurocognitive tests used in this study are well validated to detect cognitive dysfunction and examine the different cognitive domains that are particularly affected in diabetes [27, 36]. Previous studies on cognitive function and acute hypoglycaemia have generally employed fewer tests for cognitive processes. Here, we chose a test battery of seven different neurocognitive tests for a comprehensive assessment of cognitive function and focused on multiple cognitive domains associated with diabetes. The SDMT was selected as the primary outcome as it was previously proven to be sensitive to hypoglycaemia [9, 17]. This was corroborated by our results as the SDMT had the highest estimated effect size, with a value of 1.01.

Previous studies of hypoglycaemia have focused primarily on individuals with type 1 diabetes and healthy control groups. Here, we focus on type 2 diabetes as there might be differences in cognitive function between these groups. Hypoglycaemia is more frequent in asymptomatic individuals with type 2 diabetes than generally anticipated [2], making it important to clarify the effects of hypoglycaemia in this population. In this study, we demonstrate that a non-severe hypoglycaemic episode acutely impairs cognitive function. The long-term effects of non-severe glycaemic episodes have yet to be determined; however, evidence supports that severe hypoglycaemic episodes increase the risk of dementia and may cause permanent neural damage [37, 38]. Both type 1 and type 2 diabetes are associated with a greater risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia [39], but the specific mechanism underlying the relationship between glycaemic control and the complications and comorbidities of diabetes and an increased risk of dementia is unknown [40]. Since repeated hypoglycaemia itself may increase the risk of dementia [41] and since impaired cognitive function increases the risk of hypoglycaemic events [42, 43], the relationship between hypoglycaemia and dementia may be bidirectional.

Exploratory analyses showed that with poorer glycaemic control there was a greater difference in the global cognition between hypoglycaemia and euglycaemia. This contrasts with other studies in which glycaemic control did not influence the effect of hypoglycaemia (2.2 mmol/l) on cognitive function in individuals with type 1 diabetes and poor glycaemic control (mean HbA1c of 86 mmol/mol [10%]) [6]. Moreover, Korzon-Burakowska et al observed no significant difference in the glucose threshold for cognitive dysfunction when improving glycaemic control with insulin [15]. In the present study, there was no association between HbA1c and the individual cognitive domains. Furthermore, poorer glycaemic control and higher adrenaline and noradrenaline responses increased the odds for moderate/severe symptom severity during hypoglycaemia.

In all, we found that participants with higher HbA1c were more likely to have moderate/severe symptoms of hypoglycaemia and a greater difference in their global cognition performance compared with euglycaemia. Further exploratory analyses did not support a direct association between hypoglycaemia symptom severity and global cognition, suggesting that the symptoms of hypoglycaemia did not affect global cognitive performance in this population.

Further analyses of hypoglycaemic symptom severity and cognitive domains showed an association with verbal memory, as measured by RAVLT, which is an unstructured list-learning task that examines cognitive ability for learning, encoding and retrieval. None of the other cognitive domains were associated with the severity of hypoglycaemia-related symptoms. These analyses must be treated with caution since our evaluation of hypoglycaemic symptoms was based on partly objective investigator reporting. Our results suggest that several cognitive domains are affected by low levels of glucose and that the severity of hypoglycaemia-related symptoms does not influence cognitive impairment. Thus, individuals may continue with their daily activities unaware of the negative effect on cognitive performance. We suggest an increased focus on the impact of non-severe hypoglycaemia on cognition, and the potentially harmful implications on the activities of daily life.

In conclusion, acute hypoglycaemia (mean plasma glucose 3.1 mmol/l) negatively affects cognitive function in individuals with type 2 diabetes. Therefore, this degree of hypoglycaemia should be considered a substantial risk factor in the treatment of diabetes, and clinicians should attempt to minimise hypoglycaemia when aiming for a patient’s target glycaemic control.