Skip to main content
Log in

Are rules and modules really necessary for explaining language?

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Over the past few years Steven Pinker has argued that although some aspects of language may be more associational, and therefore properly modeled in connectionist networks, for the most part human language is still best characterized as a modularized set of rulesymbol systems. In support of his claim, Pinker garners a broad array of clinical, experimental, and observational data from neurology, psychology, and linguistics. Those data, unfortunately, are not compelling because they do not support his position uniquely. In this paper, I show how each of his arguments is compatible with alternative interpretations. I argue, moreover, that in focusing on certain details of connectionist models Pinker and his colleagues actually overlooked both the most serious deficiencies of the connectionist approach and its most significant theoretical contribution. I conclude by sketching briefly some emerging alternatives to connectionism which avoid those deficiencies while retaining its strengths over the rule-symbol systems of linguistic theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bellugi, U., Bihrle, A., Jernigan, T., Trauner, D., & Doherty, S. (1990). Neuropsychological, neurological, and neuroanatomical profile of Williams syndrome.American Journal of Medical Genetics Supplement, 6, 115–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, W. L. (1987). The instability of graded structures: Implications for the nature of concepts. In U. Neisser (Ed.),Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 101–140). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, W. L. (1990). On the indistinguishability of exemplar memory and abstraction in category representation. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.),Advances in social cognition (Vol. III pp. 61–88). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton, P. G. (1989). A search for explanation of the brain and learning: Elements of the psychonomic interface between psychology and neurophysiology.Psychobiology, 18, 119–161, 162–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, S. (1991). Metaphor comprehension: A connectionist approach to implications for the mental lexicon.Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 6, 227–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler, S. (1992).Language learning through experience: An exemplar-based account of language acquisition. Book manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Chandler, S. (in press).Nondeclarative linguistics: A neuropsychological perspective. To appear inRivista di Linguistica.

  • Crick, F. (1989). The recent excitement about neural networks.Nature, 337, 129–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M., Bresnan, J., & Kaplan, R. M. (1982). Acompetence-based theory of syntactic closure. In J. Bresnan (Ed.),The mental representation of grammatical relations (pp. 727–796). New York: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. B. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental language.Cognition, 39, 1–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg, S., & Stone, G. (1986). Neural dynamics of word recognition and recall: Attentional priming, learning, and resonance.Psychological Review, 93, 46–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1986). ‘Schema abstraction’ in a multiple trace memory model.Psychological Review, 93, 411–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kempley, S. T., & Morton, J. (1982). The effects of priming with regularly and irregularly related words in auditory word recognition.British Journal of Psychology, 73, 441–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruschke, J. K. (1992). ALCOVE: An examplar-based connectionist model of category learning.Psychological Review, 99, 22–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. (1970). The study of language in its social context.Studium Generale, 23, 30–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1979). Lexical insertion, inflection, and derivation: Creative processes in word production.Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 8, 477–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning.Psychological Review, 85, 207–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1989). Language acquisition. In M. I. Posner (Ed.),Foundations of cognitive science (pp. 359–399). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1991). Rules of language.Science, 253, 530–535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., & Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition.Cognition, 28, 73–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reeke, G., & Edelman, G. (1988). Real brains and artificial intelligence. In S. Graubard (Ed.),The artificial intelligence debate, false starts, real foundations (pp. 143–173). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. (1986). On learning the past tenses of English verbs. In J. McClelland, & D. Rumelhart, (Eds.),Parallel distributed processing explorations in the microstructure of congnition, Vol. 2: Psychological and biological models (pp. 216–271). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skousen, R. (1989).Analogical modeling of language. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skousen, R. (1992).Analogy and structure. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R. (1990). Content and process specificity in the effects of prior experience. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.),Advances in social cognition (Vol. III, pp. 1–60), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanners, R. F., Neiser, J. J., Hernon, W. P., & Hall, R. (1979). Memory representations for morphologically related words.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 399–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Udwin, O., & Yule, W. (1990). Expressive language of children with Williams Syndrome.American Journal of Medical Genetics Supplement, 6, 108–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1976).Wechsler intelligence scale for children-revised. New York: National Foundation for Educational Research.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This research was funded in part by University of Idaho Seed Grant 681-Y304.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chandler, S. Are rules and modules really necessary for explaining language?. J Psycholinguist Res 22, 593–606 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01072938

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01072938

Keywords

Navigation