Skip to main content

The Phonetic Realizations of the Mandarin Phoneme Inventory: The Canonical and the Variants

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Speech Perception, Production and Acquisition

Part of the book series: Chinese Language Learning Sciences ((CLLS))

Abstract

This chapter provides an overview on the phonetic realizations of the Mandarin phoneme inventory. There are two major sections. The first is a general description of the phoneme inventory, which includes five vowels and 19 consonants, along with four lexical tones. In addition to acoustic properties of individual phonemes, major allophonic rules are also discussed. The second section covers some variations on consonants, vowels, and tones in three major Mandarin varieties of Taiwan, Singapore, and China. Some variations are fairly region-specific, while others are more commonly found across various dialects. The former includes the retroflexed vowel suffix in the Mainland variety, the qualitative difference in realizing the neutral tone between the Taiwan and the Mainland variety, and the so-called fifth tone in Singapore Mandarin. The latter includes the deretroflexion and hypercorrection of sibilants, both of which can be found in Taiwan and Singapore Mandarin, and the syllable-final nasal mergers, which can be found in all three major dialects. Interestingly, these cross-dialectal variations also show large within-region variabilities. Both the canonical and the variant realizations of the phonological system are discussed in light of child language acquisition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Hanyu Pinyin is adopted for Mandarin romanization throughout this chapter.

  2. 2.

    The phone has been problematic in Mandarin phonology and has invited much discussion and debate. Classical views usually argued for two phones instead of one. For example, Norman (1988) proposed two apical vowels of [ɿ] and [ʅ], Lee-Kim (2014) argued for two syllabic approximates of [ɹ̪] and [ɻ], and Duanmu (2007) suggested two syllabic fricatives of [z̩] and [ʐ̩̩̩]. The former ones of each pair are designated to occur after dental sibilants, while the latter ones are designated to occur after retroflexes. However, since the formants in Fig. 2.3 show little frication, implying that it is at least not always realized as a syllabic fricative, and since the major difference between the two renditions of the phone ( [tsɨ] ‘Chinese character’ vs. zhì [tʂɨ] ‘mole (face)’) mainly lies in F3, not F2, implying that there is little change in tongue backness, this chapter adopts a unifying symbol [ɨ] instead, following C.-C. Cheng (1973), and views the phonetic variation as a product of coarticulation. There is a slight departure from C.-C. Cheng’s (1973) original proposal, however, as this chapter argues for a nonphonemic status instead (see Fig. 2.1).

  3. 3.

    Some researchers contended that Mandarin consonant inventory also includes an additional set of three alveolo-palatal sibilants /tɕ tɕʰ ɕ/ [e.g., Luo (1993)]. However, since these three are in complementary distribution with the velar series /k kʰ x/, the dental series /ts tsʰ s/, and the retroflexes /tʂ tʂʰ ʂ/, this chapter views them as allophones of other phonemes and discusses them in a later section.

  4. 4.

    The speakers in the Taiwan Mandarin corpus included both young (ages 20–35) and old speakers (ages 50–65) who spent all or most of their childhood and teenage years in the designated sampling locations.

  5. 5.

    An entering tone is a tone that occurs on syllables ending with /p t k ʔ/.

  6. 6.

    Zhu and Dodd (2000) reported that 75% of the children between ages 2;1 and 2;6 could produce/tʂ tʂʰ ʂ/accurately at least once, and 75% of the children reach this criterion between ages 3;1 and 3;6 for/ʐ/. Li & Munson (2016) reported about 75% accuracy for/ʂ/ at around 3;6.

References

  • Chan, H.-C. (1984). The phonetic development of Mandarin /ʐ/ in Taiwan: A sociolinguistic study. (MA), Fu Jen Catholic University, Taipei.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chao, Y. R. (1968). A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C.-Y. (1983). A fifth tone in the Mandarin spoken in Singapore. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 11(1), 92–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C.-Y. (1991). The nasal endings and retroflexed initials in Peking Mandarin: Instability and the trend of changes. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 19(2), 139–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Y., & Xu, Y. (2006). Production of weak elements in speech-evidence from F0 patterns of neutral tone in Standard Chinese. Phonetica, 63, 47–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, C.-C. (1973). A synchronic phonology of Mandarin Chinese. The Hague: De Gruyter Mouton.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng, R. L. (1966). Mandarin phonological structure. Journal of Linguistics, 2(2), 135–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chew, C. H. (2002). Xinjiapo Huayu Cihui yu Yufa (Lexicon and syntax in Singapore Mandarin). Singapore Lingzi Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chuang, Y.-Y. (2009). An acoustic study on voiceless retroflex and dental sibilants in Taiwan Mandarin spontaneous speech. (M.A. thesis), National Taiwan University, Taipei.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chuang, Y.-Y., Chiang, Y.-J., & Fon, J. (2012). The effects of context and Min dialect on the realizations of /ʐ/ variants in Taiwan Mandarin. Paper presented at the 2nd Workshop on Sound Change, Kloster Seeon, Germany.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chuang, Y.-Y., & Fon, J. (2010). The effect of prosodic prominence on the realizations of voiceless dental and retroflex sibilants in Taiwan Mandarin spontaneous speech. Paper presented at the 5th International Conference on Speech Prosody.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chuang, Y.-Y., Wang, S.-F., & Fon, J. (2015). Cross-linguistic interaction between two voiced fricatives in Mandarin-Min simultaneous bilinguals. Paper presented at the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Glasgow, Scotland, U.K.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chung, K. S. (2006). Hypercorrection in Taiwan Mandarin. Journal of Asian Pacific Communication, 16(2), 197–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duanmu, S. (2007). The phonology of standard Chinese (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fon, J. (2004). A preliminary construction of Taiwan Southern Min spontaneous speech corpus (NSC-92–2411-H-003–050-).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fon, J., Hung, J.-M., Huang, Y.-H., & Hsu, H.-J. (2011). Dialectal variations on syllable-final nasal mergers in Taiwan Mandarin. Language and Linguistics, 12(2), 273–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartman, L. M. (1944). The segmental phonemes of the Peiping dialect. Language, 20(1), 28–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • He, K., & Li, D. (1987). Xiandai Hanyu San Qian Changyong Ci Biao [Three Thousand Most Commonly Used Words in Modern Chinese]. Beijing: Beijing Shifan Daxue Chubanshe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, S. (1993). Yuyan, Shehui yu Zuqun Yishi—Taiwan yuyan shehuixue de yanjiu [Language, society, and ethnic identity: Studies in language sociology in Taiwan]. Taipei: Crane Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubler, C. C. (1985a). The development of Mandarin in Taiwan: A case study of language contact. Taipei: Student Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubler, C. C. (1985b). The influence of Southern Min on the Mandarin of Taiwan. Anthropological Linguistics, 27(2), 156–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee-Kim, S.-I. (2014). Revisiting Mandarin ‘apical vowels’: An articulatory and acoustic study. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 44(3), 261–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, F., & Munson, B. (2016). The development of voiceless sibilant fricatives in Putonghua-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 59(4), 699–712.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, H.-B. (1982). Comparison of the differences in tone sandhi among slow speech, normal speech and fast speech in Mandarin Chinese. (M.A. thesis), The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Y.-H. (1989). Autosegmental treatment of segmental process in Chinese phonology. (Ph.D. dissertation), University of Texas at Austin, Austin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luo, C.-C. (1993). Kuoyuxue [Studies of Mandarin Chinese]. Taipei: Wu-Nan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maddieson, I. (1984). The UCLA phonological segment inventory database. Retrieved 4 June 2015. https://web.phonetik.uni-frankfurt.de/upsid.html.

  • Maddieson, I. (2013). Tone. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ng, B. C. (1985). A study of the variable /sh/ on Singapore Mandarin. In D. Bradley (Ed.), Language policy, language planning and sociolinguistics in South-East Asia (pp. 31–37). Canberra, Australia: Pacific Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, J. (1988). Chinese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peng, S.-H., Chan, M. K. M., Tseng, C.-Y., Huang, T., Lee, O. J., & Beckman, E. M. (2005). Towards a pan-Mandarin system for prosodic transcription. In S.-A. Jun (Ed.), Prosodic typology: The phonology of intonation and phrasing (pp. 230–270). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Population Reference Bureau 2013. 2013 World Population Data Sheet. Population Reference Bureau.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wan, I.-P., & Jaeger, J. (2003). The phonological representation of Taiwan Mandarin vowels: A psycholinguistic study. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 12(3), 205–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Y. (1994). Mandarin segmental phonology. (Ph.D. dissertation), University of Toronto, Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, J.H.-T. (2010). Phonetic evidence for the nasal coda shift in Mandarin. Taiwan Journal of Linguistics, 8(1), 29–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, J., & Chen, Y. (2014). 3rd tone sandhi in Standard Chinese: A corpus approach. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 42(1), 218–237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, H., & Dodd, B. (2000). The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (Modern Standard Chinese). Journal of Child Language, 27(1), 3–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Janice Fon .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Fon, J. (2020). The Phonetic Realizations of the Mandarin Phoneme Inventory: The Canonical and the Variants. In: Liu, H., Tsao, F., Li, P. (eds) Speech Perception, Production and Acquisition. Chinese Language Learning Sciences. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-7606-5_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-7606-5_2

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-15-7605-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-15-7606-5

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics