Skip to main content

Constructing the Cooperative Niche

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Entangled Life

Part of the book series: History, Philosophy and Theory of the Life Sciences ((HPTL,volume 4))

Abstract

Humans contrast with their great ape relatives in many ways, but one of the most striking is our richly cooperative social lives. The explanation of this difference is complex and multi-factorial. But this paper argues that one central element is niche construction. Hominins are inveterate and extensive niche constructors. Individually and collectively, we have deeply affected our physical and biological environment, and have used technology to filter and transform the selective effects of the changed physical and biology worlds in which we have lived. But members of our lineage have not just acted on physical and biological environments; they have organised their informational environment too. Not just their own, but that of the next generation. While intensive and active teaching is probably a recent phenomenon, teaching itself is not. Furthermore, adults structure the learning environment of the next generation in many other ways: by acting as models of adult life; by providing supervised, safer environments; by providing toys, tools and props that structure and support trial and error learning. So the skills, values, ideas, information, and expected modes of social interaction and behaviour are made accessible to the next generation. This happens in circumstances which have often been adapted to enhance learning. The main theme of this paper is to show that humans cooperate more than other great apes largely because they reconstruct their environment more than other great apes, and one aspect of that reconstruction has been to make a world in which cooperation could survive and expand.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    There is a rich archaeological literature on “behavioral modernity,” i.e., on the archaeological signs that ancient foragers resemble those known from ethnography. An important issue is the fact that these signs are about 100,000 years younger than our species (d’Errico 2003; Henshilwood and Marean 2003; Nowell 2010; Sterelny 2011). I interpret many of these signs of modernity as signs that a reciprocation-based economy is replacing a mutualistic economy.

  2. 2.

    If the group did not forage as a single unit, those who found a major kill or killing opportunity would need to recruit others; power scavenging offers large economies of scale.

  3. 3.

    Wynn and Coolidge have long argued that hominin cognitive evolution is largely an expansion of working memory. For them, working memory seems to include executive function skills: planned behaviour and the capacity to resist distraction (Wynn and Coolidge 2004, 2010).

  4. 4.

    By this I mean sapiens, Neanderthals, and their immediate predecessor: the very large-brained hominins of the last half a million years.

  5. 5.

    Don Ross goes further, arguing that humans shape one another’s psychology and habits, creating in one another stable and relatively public intentional profiles, making longer term collaboration and coordination possible. Our unshaped brains would leave us with much less stable world views and preference functions, and hence make our moment-by-moment decision making far less predictable (Ross 2006a).

  6. 6.

    That is not all they do, of course, and while these contemporary social mechanisms do coordinate and regularize interaction, making forms of cooperation possible that would be otherwise inconceivable, they also distribute the profits of those interactions very unequally, while often at the same time entrenching those inequalities.

References

  • Barnard, Alan. 2011. Social anthropology and human origins. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham, Paul. 1999. Human uniqueness: A general theory. Quarterly Review of Biology 74: 133–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham, Paul. 2000. Human evolution and human history: A complete theory. Evolutionary Anthropology 9: 248–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, Chris. 1999. Hierarchy in the forest. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, Christopher. 2012. Moral origins: The evolution of virtue, altruism and shame. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogucki, Peter. 1999. The origins of human society. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, Sam, and Herbert Gintis. 2011. A cooperative species: Human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burkart, Judith, Sarah Blaffer Hrdy, and Carel van Schaik. 2009. Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology 18: 175–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R. 1995. The thinking ape: Evolutionary origins of intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, Richard. 2000. Evolution of primate cognition. Cognitive Science 24: 543–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, Richard. 2002. Seeing actions as hierarchically organized structures: Great ape manual skills. In The imitative mind, ed. Andrew Meltzoff and William Prinz, 122–140. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, Richard. 2004. The manual skills and cognition that lie behind hominid tool use. In Evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence, ed. Anne Russon and David R. Begun, 31–44. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Carmody, Rachael, and Richard Wrangham. 2009. The energetic significance of cooking. Journal of Human Evolution 57: 379–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, Gergely, and György Gergely. 2011. Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 1149–1157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F. 2003. The invisible frontier: A multiple species model for the origin of behavioural modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 188–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, Franz. 2008. Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes, 25th anniversary ed. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, Franz, and Malini Suchak. 2010. Prosocial primates: Selfish and unselfish motivations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 365: 2711–2722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flannery, Kent, and Joyce Marcus. 2012. The creation of inequality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley, Robert. 1995. Humans before humanity. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley, Robert, and Clive Gamble. 2009. The ecology of social transitions in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 364: 3267–3279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foley, Robert, and Marta Mirazon Lahr. 2003. On stony ground: Lithic technology, human evolution, and the emergence of culture. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 109–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurven, Michael, Hillard Kaplan, and Maguin Gutierrez. 2006. How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of extended development and long life span. Journal of Human Evolution 51: 454–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, Kristin. 2003. Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity. American Journal of Human Biology 15: 380–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., J.F. O’Connell, N.G. Blurton Jones, H. Alvarez, and E. Charnov. 1998. Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95: 1336–1339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, Christopher, and Curtis Marean. 2003. The origin of modern behavior. Current Anthropology 44: 627–651 (includes peer commentary and author’s responses).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewlett, Barry, Hillary Fouts, Adam Boyette, and Bonnie Hewlett. 2011. Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 1168–1178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hrdy, Sarah Blaffer. 2009. Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, Nicholas. 1976. The social function of intellect. In Growing points in ethology, ed. P.P.G. Bateson and R.A. Hinde, 303–317. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, Martin. 2007. Feast: Why humans share food. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joyce, Richard. 2006. Evolution of morality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, Hilliard, Paul Hooper, and Michael Gurven. 2009. The evolutionary and ecological roots of human social organization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 364: 3289–3299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher, Philip. 2011. The ethical project. Harvard: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, Richard G. 2009a. Darwin and the recent African origin of modern humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106: 16007–16009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, Richard G. 2009b. The human career: Human biological and cultural origins, 3rd ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty, Sally. 2007. Down with the revolution. In Rethinking the human revolution: new behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans, ed. K. Paul Mellars, Bar-Yosef O. Boyle, and C. Stringer, 133–151. Cambridge: McDonald Institute Archaeological Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty, S., and A. Brooks. 2000. The revolution that wasn’t: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution 39: 453–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McPherron, Shannon, Zeresenay Alemseged, Curtis Marean, Jonathan Wynn, Denné Reed, Denis Geraads, René Bobe, and Hamdallah Béarat. 2010. Evidence for stone-tool-assisted consumption of animal tissues before 3.39 million years ago at Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature 466: 857–860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowell, April. 2010. Defining behavioral modernity in the context of Neandertal and anatomically modern human populations. Annual Review of Anthropology 39: 437–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J.F., K. Hawkes, and N.G. Blurton Jones. 1999. Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution 36: 461–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee, F. John, Kevin Laland, and Marcus Feldman. 2003. Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution, Monographs in population biology. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Okasha, Samir. 2006. Evolution and the units of selection. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, Adam, Stephen Shennan, and Mark Thomas. 2009. Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324: 1298–1301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebroeks, Will (ed.). 2007. Guts and brains: An integrative approach to the hominin record. Leiden: Leiden University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, Don. 2006a. The economic and evolutionary basis of selves. Cognitive Systems Research 7: 246–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, Don. 2006b. Evolutionary game theory and the normative theory of institutional design: Binmore and behavioral economics. Politics, Philosophy and Economics 5: 51–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seabright, Paul. 2010. The company of strangers: A natural history of economic life. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw-Williams, Kim. 2011. The triggering track-ways theory. M.A. thesis, Victoria University of Wellington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, Kim. 2011. From hominins to humans: How sapiens became behaviourally modern. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366: 809–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, Kim. 2012a. The evolved apprentice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, Kim. 2012b. Morality’s dark past. Analyse and Kritik 34: 95–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, Kim. 2013a. Cooperation in a complex world: The role of proximate factors in ultimate explanations. Biological Theory 7: 358–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, Kim. 2013b. Life in interesting times: Cooperation and collective action in the Holocene. In Cooperation and its evolution, eds. Kim Sterelny, Richard Joyce, Brett Calcott, and Ben Fraser, 89–108. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, Kim. Forthcoming. Signals, symbols and norms. Biological Theory.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiner, Mary C. 2002. Carnivory, coevolution, and the geographic spread of the genus Homo. Journal of Archaeological Research 10: 1–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stout, Dietrich. 2011. Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Series B 366: 1050–1059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, Michael. 2009. Why we cooperate, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, Michael, and Malinda Carpenter. 2007. Shared intentionality. Developmental Science 10: 121–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, Michael, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne, and Henrike Moll. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 675–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, Michael, Alicia P. Melis, Claudio Tennie, Emily Wyman, and Esther Herrmann. 2012. Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology 53: 673–692.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. Scott. 2000. The extended organism: The physiology of animal-built structures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadley, Lyn. 2010. Compound-adhesive manufacture as a behavioral proxy for complex cognition in the Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology 51: S111–S119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warneken, Felix, and Michael Tomasello. 2009. Varieties of altruism in children and chimpanzees. Trends in Cognitive Science 13: 397–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warneken, Felix, Brian Hare, Alicia Melis, Daniel Hanus, and Michael Tomasello. 2007. Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLoS Biology 5: e184. doi:110.1371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A., and R. Byrne (eds.). 1997. Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, Gerald S. 1990. Food sharing in vampire bats. Scientific American 262: 64–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, David Sloan. 2002. Darwin’s cathedral: Evolution, religion and the nature of society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, Richard. 2001. Out of the pan, into the fire: How our ancestors’ evolution depended on what they ate. In Tree of life, ed. Franz de Waal, 121–143. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, Richard. 2009. Catching fire: How cooking made us human. London: Profile Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, Thomas, and Frederick Coolidge. 2004. The expert Neanderthal mind. Journal of Human Evolution 46: 467–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, Thomas, and Frederick Coolidge. 2010. How Levallois reduction is similar to, and not similar to, playing chess. In Stone tools and the evolution of human cognition, ed. April Nowell and Iain Davidson, 83–103. Boulder: University of Colorado Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kim Sterelny .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sterelny, K. (2014). Constructing the Cooperative Niche. In: Barker, G., Desjardins, E., Pearce, T. (eds) Entangled Life. History, Philosophy and Theory of the Life Sciences, vol 4. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-7067-6_13

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics