Skip to main content

Research Design: Toward a Realistic Role for Causal Analysis

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of Causal Analysis for Social Research

Part of the book series: Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research ((HSSR))

Abstract

For a half-century, sociology and allied social sciences have worked with a model of research design founded on a distinction between internal validity, the capacity of designs to support statements about cause and effect, and external validity, the extent to which the results from specific studies can be generalized beyond the batch of data on which they are founded. The distinction is conceptually useful and has great pedagogic value, that is, the association of the experimental model with internal validity, and random sampling with external validity. The advent of the potential outcomes model of causation, by emphasizing the definition of a causal effect at the unit level and the heterogeneity of causal effects, has made it clear how indistinct (and interpenetrated) are these “twin pillars” of research design. This is the theme of this chapter, which inveighs against the idea of a hierarchy of research design desiderata, with causal inference at the peak. Rather, I adopt the design typology of Leslie Kish (1987), which advocates an appropriate balance of randomization, representation, and realism, and illustrate how all three elements (and not just randomization, the internal validity design mechanism) are integrated aspects of meaningful causal analysis. What is meaningful causal analysis? It depends first and foremost on getting straight why we are doing what we are doing. Understanding why something has happened may tell us a lot about what will happen if we were actually to do something, but this is not necessarily so.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 99.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    My treatment is necessarily selective. These are not the only eminent statisticians who have put research design at the forefront of thinking about causation.

  2. 2.

    It is less well known that his Ph.D. was in sociology, part of a fascinating intellectual and personal background (Frankel and King 1996).

  3. 3.

    This includes the possible overrepresentation of certain domains for their theoretical salience, their meager share of the population notwithstanding (Smith 1990: 68).

  4. 4.

    Bollen and Pearl (Chap. 15, this volume) take explicit issue with Freedman’s characterizations of, in particular, recursive path models.

  5. 5.

    A well-known piece of Freedman’s mockery—“The Modelers’ Responses”—appears on the same page.

  6. 6.

    If the latter, then this is a meal that, from the get-go, sociologists have been disinclined to eat on its own (Chap. 2 by Barringer, Leahey, and Eliason, this volume).

  7. 7.

    This is related to the issue of support for inference, mentioned above, and also to the manipulation criterion, to be discussed below (Smith 1997: 333–334).

  8. 8.

    The related question of what an experiment does or does not tell us about causal mechanisms will be taken up two sections hence, in The Experiment as the Model for Research Design. The definitions of causal mechanisms here, as per Rosenbaum (1984: 42), differ in some ways from canonical sociological treatments of causal mechanisms and theory (e.g., Hedström and Swedberg 1998). Attempts to either integrate or differentiate these perspectives appear in Goldthorpe (2001), Morgan and Winship (2007: 219–242), Smith (n.d.: 33–35), and—especially—Knight and Winship (Chap. 14, this volume).

  9. 9.

    Kish (1987), for example, incorporates measures of bias, of stochastic (e.g., sampling) variation, and of cost (fixed and unit-specific) in the same equations, hence in comparable metrics.

  10. 10.

    Paternal age—which is strongly related to maternal age—may be the biological source of the de novo mutations (Shelton et al. 2010; O’Roak et al. 2012; Kong et al. 2012).

  11. 11.

    The “uniformity among units” is with respect to the effect of a treatment.

  12. 12.

    There are discussions of heterogeneity and interactions with respect to treatment effects in the foundational work on randomized experiments (Fisher [1925] 1951). But heterogeneity was with reference to variance among subjects in other factors related to the response but independent of assignment to treatment, hence on the efficiency of experimental designs (pp. 107–109), and interactions in effects of factors were with respect to other factors in the design of the experiment, not to characteristics of the units subject to randomization (e.g., pp. 93–99).

  13. 13.

    Either that or that it is only white male psychiatrists who know how to use correctly this information on the empirical incidence of psychological disorders conditional on the gender and race of the patient.

  14. 14.

    This is the simplest structure of causal mediation analysis by Wang and Sobel (Chap. 12, this volume). Their chapter provides a detailed formal exposition of most of the causal issues posed by the various studies illustrated in Table 4.2.

  15. 15.

    I am indebted to Xiaolu Wang for developing, clarifying, and drawing out the following points.

  16. 16.

    Thus, parolees could substitute these payments for the money that would otherwise be derived from work. In contrast, charter schools do not require additional subvention, so the conditional cash transfer is a net plus for those who are offered it and send their children to charter schools. This distinguishes the charter school experiment from a similar conditional cash transfer scheme in which vouchers that can be used to pay Catholic school fees are randomly tendered (Morgan and Winship 2007).

  17. 17.

    This is without considering the possibility that heterogeneity in treatment effects obtains not only with respect to families but also to school types—what Morgan and Winship (2012) term compositional heterogeneity.

  18. 18.

    “No causation without manipulation” is the third of eight “myths” addressed by Bollen and Pearl (Chap. 15, this volume). They tend to hold with the critics of the manipulation criterion, but at the core their myth busting targets the irrelevance of this criterion for the practice of causal analysis via structural equation models.

References

  • Ackoff, R. L. (1953). The design of social research. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angrist, J. D., Imbens, G. W., & Rubin, D. B. (1996). Identification of causal effects using instrumental variables. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 91(434), 444–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arceneaux, K., Gerber, A. S., & Green, D. P. (2010). A cautionary note on the use of matching to estimate causal effects: An empirical example comparing matching estimates to an experimental benchmark. Sociological Methods & Research, 39(2), 256–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Babbie, E. (2010). The practice of social research (12th ed.). Belmont: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berk, R. A. (1991). Toward a methodology for mere mortals. In P. V. Marsden (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 315–324). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berk, R. A. (2005). Randomized experiments as the bronze standard. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 1(4), 417–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berk, R. A., & Sherman, L. W. (1988). Police responses to family violence incidents: An analysis of an experimental design with incomplete randomization. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(401), 70–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickel, P. J., & Freedman, D. A. (1981). Some asymptotic theory for the bootstrap. The Annals of Statistics, 9(6), 1196–1217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blalock, H. M., Jr. (1991). Are there really any constructive alternatives to causal modeling. In P. V. Marsden (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 325–335). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bongaarts, J., & Potter, R. G. (1983). Fertility, biology, and behavior: An analysis of the proximate determinants. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boruch, R. (Ed.). (2005). Place randomized trials: Experimental tests of public policy. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 599.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brand, J. E., & Xie, Y. (2007). Identification and estimation of causal effects with time-varying treatments and time-varying outcomes. In Y. Xie (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 393–434). Boston/Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for research. Chicago: Rand McNally & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Card, D. (1999). The causal effect of education on earnings. In O. Ashenfelter & D. Card (Eds.), Handbook of labor economics (Vol. 5, pp. 1801–1863). New York: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheslack-Postava, K., Liu, K., & Bearman, P. S. (2011). Closely spaced pregnancies are associated with increased odds of autism in California sibling births. Pediatrics, 127(2), 246–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T. D. (2002). Randomized experiments in educational policy research: A critical examination of the reasons the educational evaluation community has offered for not doing them. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 24(3), 175–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings. Chicago: Rand McNally & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J. (1982). Designing evaluations of educational and social programs. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawid, A. P. (2000). Causal inference without counterfactuals. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 95(450), 407–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawid, A. P., & Fienberg, S. E. (2011, July). The causes of effects. In Plenary talk presented at 8th international conference on forensic inference statistics, Seattle, WA.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Brauw, A., & Hoddinott, J. (2011). Must conditional cash transfer programs be conditioned to be effective? The impact of conditioning transfers on school enrollment in Mexico. Journal of Development Economics, 96(2), 359–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diaconis, P., & Freedman, D. (1986). On the consistency of Bayes estimates. The Annals of Statistics, 14(1), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, O. D. (1974). Developing social indicators. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 71(12), 5096–5102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, O. D. (1984). Notes on social measurement: Historical and critical. New York: Russell Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farewell, V. T. (1979). Some results on the estimation of logistic models based on retrospective data. Biometrika, 66(1), 27–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Firebaugh, G. (1978). A rule for inferring individual-level relationships from aggregate data. American Sociological Review, 43(4), 557–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. A. ([1925] 1951). Statistical methods for research workers (6th ed.). New York: Hafner Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. A. ([1935] 1958). The design of experiments (13th ed.). New York: Hafner Publishing Company, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fountain, C., & Bearman, P. (2011). Risk as social context: Immigration policy and autism. Sociological Forum, 26(2), 215–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frangakis, C. E., & Rubin, D. B. (2002). Principal stratification in causal inference. Biometrics, 58(1), 21–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frankel, M., & King, B. (1996). A conversation with Leslie Kish. Statistical Science, 11(1), 65–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. A. (1981). Bootstrapping regression models. The Annals of Statistics, 9(6), 1218–1228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. A. (1983). Markov chains. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. A. (1985). Statistics and the scientific method. In W. M. Mason & S. E. Fienberg (Eds.), Cohort analysis in social research: Beyond the identification problem (pp. 343–366). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. A. (1991a). Statistical models and shoe leather. In P. V. Marsden (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1991 (pp. 291–313). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. A. (1991b). A rejoinder to Berk, Blalock, and Mason. In P. V. Marsden (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1991 (pp. 353–358). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D. A. (2005). Statistical models: Theory and practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Freedman, D., Pisani, R., & Purves, R. (1978). Statistics. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gage, N. L. (Ed.). (1963). Handbook of research on teaching. Chicago: Rand McNally & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gangl, M. (2010). Causal inference in sociological research. Annual Review of Sociology, 36, 21–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldthorpe, J. H. (2001). Causation, statistics, and sociology. European Sociological Review, 17(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greiner, D. J., & Rubin, D. B. (2011). Causal effects of perceived immutable characteristics. The Review of Economics and Statistics, 93(3), 775–785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Härdle, W. (1990). Applied nonparametric regression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. J. (2001). Micro data, heterogeneity, and the evaluation of public policy: Nobel lecture. Journal of Political Economy, 109(4), 673–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckman, J. J., & Smith, J. A. (1995). Assessing the case for social experiments. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 9(2), 85–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedström, P., & Swedberg, R. (1998). Social mechanisms: An introductory essay. In P. Hedström & R. Swedberg (Eds.), Social mechanisms: An analytical approach to social theory (pp. 1–31). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P. W. (1986). Statistics and causal inference. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 81(396), 945–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, P. W. (2008). Causation and race. In T. Zuberi & E. Bonilla-Silva (Eds.), White logic, white methods: Racism and methodology (pp. 93–109). Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jick, H., & Kaye, J. A. (2003). Epidemiology and possible causes of autism. Pharmacotherapy, 23(12), 1524–1530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Hanks, J. A., Bachrach, C. A., Morgan, S. P., & Kohler, H.-P. (2011). Understanding family change and variation: Toward a theory of conjunctural action. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kadane, J. B. (2011). Principles of uncertainty. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • King, M., & Bearman, P. (2009). Diagnostic change and the increased prevalence of autism. International Journal of Epidemiology, 38(5), 1224–1234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, M. D., Fountain, C., Dakhlallah, D., & Bearman, P. S. (2009). Estimated autism and older reproductive age. American Journal of Public Health, 99(9), 1673–1679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kish, L. (1965). Survey sampling. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kish, L. (1987). Statistical design for research. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kong, A., Frigge, M. L., Masson, G., Besenbacher, S., Sulem, P., Magnusson, G., Gudjonsson, S. A., Sigurdsson, A., Jonasdottir, A., Jonasdottir, A., Wong, W. S. W., Sigurdsson, G., Walters, G. B., Steinberg, S., Helgason, H., Thorleifsson, G., Gudbjartsson, D. F., Helgason, A., Magnusson, O. T., Thorsteinsdottir, U., & Stefansson, K. (2012). Rate of de novo mutations and the importance of father’s age to disease risk. Nature, 488, 471–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberson, S., & Lynn, F. B. (2002). Barking up the wrong branch: Scientific alternatives to the current model of sociological science. Annual Review of Sociology, 28, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, K.-Y., King, M., & Bearman, P. S. (2010a). Social influence and the autism epidemic. The American Journal of Sociology, 115(5), 1387–1434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, K., Zerubavel, N., & Bearman, P. (2010b). Social demographic change and autism. Demography, 47(2), 327–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loring, M., & Powell, B. (1988). Gender, race, and DSM-III: A study of objectivity of psychiatric diagnostic behavior. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 29(1), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ludwig, J., Liebman, J. B., Kling, J. R., Duncan, G. J., Katz, L. F., Kessler, R. C., & Sanbonmatsu, L. (2008). What can we learn about neighborhood effects from the moving to opportunity experiment? The American Journal of Sociology, 114(1), 144–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marini, M. M., & Singer, B. (1988). Causality in the social sciences. In C. C. Clogg (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1988 (pp. 347–409). Washington, DC: American Sociological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merli, M. G., Qian, Z., & Smith, H. L. (2004). Adaptation of a political bureaucracy to economic and institutional change under socialism: The Chinese state family planning system. Politics and Society, 31(2), 231–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mill, J. S. (1843). A system of logic, ratiocinative and inductive, being a connected view of the principles of evidence, and the methods of scientific investigation. London: John W. Parker.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, S. L., & Winship, C. (2007). Counterfactuals and causal inference: Methods and principles for social research. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, S. L. & Winship, C. (2012). Bringing context and variability back in to causal analysis, Chapter 14. In H. Kincaid (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of the philosophy of the social sciences. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ní Bhrolcháin, M., & Dyson, T. (2007). On causation in demography: Issues and illustrations. Population and Development Review, 33(1), 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Roak, B. J., Vives, L., Girirajan, S., Karakoc, E., Krumm, N., Coe, B. P., Levy, R., Ko, A., Lee, C., Smith, J. D., Turner, E. H., Stanaway, I. B., Vernot, B., Malig, M., Baker, C., Reilly, B., Akey, J. M., Borenstein, E., Rieder, M. J., Nickerson, D. A., Bernier, R., Shendure, J., & Eichler, E. E. (2012). Sporadic autism exomes reveal a highly interconnected protein network of de novo mutations. Nature, 485(7397), 246–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, P. R. (1984). From association to causation in observational studies: The role of tests of strongly ignorable treatment assignment. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 79, 41–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, P. R. (2002). Observational studies (2nd ed.). New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, P. R. (2009). Design of observational studies. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, P. R., & Rubin, D. B. (1983a). The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika, 70(1), 41–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, P. R., & Rubin, D. B. (1983b). Assessing sensitivity to an unobserved binary covariate in an observational study with binary outcome. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 45(2), 212–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossi, P. H., Berk, R. A., & Lenihan, K. J. (1982). Saying it wrong with figures: A comment on Zeisel. The American Journal of Sociology, 88(2), 390–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. B. (1974). Estimating causal effects of treatments in randomized and nonrandomized studies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66(5), 688–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, D. B. (2005). Causal inference using potential outcomes: Design, modeling, decisions. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 100(469), 322–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russo, F., Wunsch, G., & Mouchart, M. (2010). Inferring causality through counterfactuals in observational studies: Some epistemological issues (Discussion Paper 1029). Institut de statistique, biostatistique et sciences actuarielles (ISBA), Université Catholique de Louvain. http://www.stat.ucl.ac.be/ISpub/dp/2010/DP1029.pdf

  • Rytand, D. A. (1980). Sutton’s or dock’s Law. The New England Journal of Medicine, 302(17), 972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, R. J. (2010). Gold standard myths: Observations on the experimental turn in quantitative criminology. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 26(4), 489–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston/New York: Houghton Mifflin Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shelton, J. F., Tancredi, D. J., & Hertz-Picciotto, I. (2010). Independent and dependent contributions of advanced maternal and paternal ages to autism risk. Autism Research, 3(1), 30–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloan, J. H., Kellermann, A. L., Reay, D. T., Ferris, J. A., Koepsell, T., Rivara, F. P., Rice, C., Gray, L., & LoGerfo, J. (1988). Handgun regulations, crime, assaults, and homicide: A tale of two cities. The New England Journal of Medicine, 319(19), 1256–1262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. L. (1990). Specification problems in experimental and nonexperimental social research. In C. C. Clogg (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1990 (pp. 59–91). Cambridge, MA: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. L. (1997). Matching with multiple controls to estimate treatment effects in observational studies. In A. E. Raftery (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1997 (pp. 325–353). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. L. (2003). Some thoughts on causation as it relates to demography and population studies. Population and Development Review, 29(3), 459–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. L. (2005). Introducing new contraceptives in rural China: A field experiment. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 599, 246–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. L. (2009). Causation and its discontents. In H. Engelhardt-Woelfler, H.-P. Kohler, & A. Fuernkranz-Prskawetz (Eds.), Causal analysis in population studies: Concepts, methods, applications. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, H. L. (n.d.). La causalité en sociologie et démographie. Retour sur le principe de laction humaine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, M. E. (2006). What do randomized studies of housing mobility demonstrate? Causal inference in the face of interference. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 101(476), 1398–1407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stinchcombe, A. L. (1969). Constructing social theories. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tukey, J. (1986). Sunset salvo. The American Statistician, 40(1), 72–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaupel, J. W., Carey, J. R., & Christensen, K. (2003). It’s never too late. Science, 301(5640), 1679–1681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, T., & Kluge, F. (2012, May 5). Does public spending level mortality inequalities? — Findings from East Germany after unification. In Presented at the annual meeting of the Population Association of America, San Francisco, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, T., Vaupel, J. W., & Rau, R. (2012). Health or wealth. Life expectancy convergence after the German unification. Dissertation, Max Planck Institute for Demographic Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wainer, H. (Ed.). ([1986] 2000). Drawing inferences from self-selected samples. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheaton, B. (1978). The sociogenesis of psychological disorder: Reexamining the causal issues with longitudinal data. American Sociological Review, 43(3), 383–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winship, C., & Morgan, S. L. (1999). The estimation of causal effects from observational data. Annual Review of Sociology, 25, 659–706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeisel, H. (1982a). Disagreement over the evaluation of a controlled experiment. American Journal of Sociology, 88(2), 378–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeisel, H. (1982b). Hans Zeisel concludes the debate. American Journal of Sociology, 88(2), 394–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Herbert L. Smith .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Smith, H.L. (2013). Research Design: Toward a Realistic Role for Causal Analysis. In: Morgan, S. (eds) Handbook of Causal Analysis for Social Research. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-6094-3_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics