Skip to main content

Divine Understanding and the Divided Brain

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:
Handbook of Neuroethics

Abstract

Interaction with the world requires the right hemisphere’s broad attention, which is inclusive and opens up into possibility, coupled with the left hemisphere’s narrow attention, which collapses the world we experience into specificity. If the left hemisphere collapses the world too quickly into what is specific, however, it precludes the possibility of knowledge that transcends what is already familiar, notably purported knowledge of the divine. By contrast, the right hemisphere is more sensitive to image, metaphor, and narratives by which theological knowledge may be capable of expression that would be ambiguous or apparently contradictory if expressed simply as a set of propositions. The reciprocal organization of the cerebral hemispheres therefore suggests that any proposed theology that is articulated simply in terms of a set of specific propositions about the divine risks betraying, distorting, and misrepresenting its subject matter. Furthermore, this organization of brain structure and function suggests that images, metaphors, and narratives are not poor substitutes or intermediate steps in theological knowledge, but indispensable to it.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 999.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 999.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aboitiz, F., Scheibel, A. B., & Zaidel, E. (1992). Morphometry of the Sylvian fissure and the corpus callosum, with emphasis on sex differences. Brain, 115(5), 1521–1541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adair, J. C., Schwartz, R. L., & Barrett, A. M. (2003). Anosognosia. In K. M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (4th ed., pp. 185–214). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, M. P., Benson, D. F., & Stuss, D. T. (1989). Frontal lobes and language. Brain and Language, 37(4), 656–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allison, J. D., Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., et al. (2000). Functional MRI cerebral activation and deactivation during finger movement. Neurology, 54(1), 135–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrew, R. J., & Rogers, L. J. (2002). The nature of lateralisation in tetrapods. In R. J. Andrew & L. J. Rogers (Eds.), Comparative vertebrate lateralisation (pp. 94–125). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banich, M. T. (2003). Interaction between the hemispheres and its implications for the processing capacity of the brain. In K. Hugdahl & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The asymmetrical brain (pp. 261–302). Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bender, M. B., Feldman, M., & Sobin, A. J. (1968). Palinopsia. Brain, 9(2), 321–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisazza, A., Cantalupo, C., Capocchiano, M., et al. (2000). Population lateralisation and social behaviour: A study with sixteen species of fish. Laterality, 5(3), 269–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiacchi, P., Marzi, C. A., & Nicoletti, R. (1994). Left-right asymmetry of callosal transfer in normal human subjects. Behavioural Brain Research, 64(1–2), 173–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bisiach, E., Vallar, G., Perani, D., et al. (1986). Unawareness of disease following lesions of the right hemisphere: Anosognosia for hemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianopia. Neuropsychologia, 24(4), 471–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blakeslee, T. R. (1980). The right brain. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blonder, L. X., Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). The role of the right hemisphere in emotional communication. Brain, 114(3), 1115–1127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, B., & Kidron, D. P. (1959). Prosopagnosia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 22(2), 124–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, B., Sroka, H., & Munitz, H. (1969). Prosopagnosia with animal face agnosia. Cortex, 5(2), 164–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borod, J. C. (1993). Cerebral mechanisms underlying facial, prosodic, and lexical emotional expression: A review of neuropsychological studies and methodological issues. Neuropsychology, 7, 445–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bottini, G., Corcoran, R., Sterzi, R., et al. (1994). The role of the right hemisphere in the interpretation of figurative aspects of language: A positron emission tomography activation study. Brain, 117(6), 1241–1253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourgeois, M. J., Christman, S., & Horowitz, I. A. (1998). The role of hemispheric activation in person perception: Evidence for an attentional focus model. Brain and Cognition, 38(2), 202–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breitenstein, C., Daum, I., & Ackermann, H. (1998). Emotional processing following cortical and subcortical brain damage: Contribution of the fronto-striatal circuitry. Behavioural Neurology, 11(1), 29–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, H. D., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1993). Cerebral lateralisation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 3(2), 183–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S., & Nicholls, M. E. (1997). Hemispheric asymmetries for the temporal resolution of brief auditory stimuli. Perception and Psychophysics, 59(3), 442–447.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, W. S., Larson, E. B., & Jeeves, M. (1994). Directional asymmetries in interhemispheric transmission time: Evidence from visual evoked potentials. Neuropsychologia, 32(4), 439–448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, H. H., Potter, H. H., Bihrle, A. M., et al. (1986). Inference deficits in right brain-damaged patients. Brain and Language, 27(2), 310–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bugnyar, T., Stöwe, M., & Heinrich, B. (2004). Ravens, Corvus corax, follow gaze direction of humans around obstacles. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 271(1546), 1331–1336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmon, A., & Nachshon, I. (1971). Effects of unilateral brain damage on the perception of temporal order. Cortex, 7(4), 411–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carmon, A., & Nachshon, I. (1973). Ear asymmetry in perception of emotional nonverbal stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 37(6), 351–357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiarello, C., & Maxfield, L. (1996). Varieties of interhemispheric inhibition, or how to keep a good hemisphere down. Brain and Cognition, 30(1), 81–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conti, F., & Manzoni, T. (1994). The neurotransmitters and postsynaptic actions of callosally projecting neurons. Behavioural Brain Research, 64(1–2), 37–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, N. D. (1984). Homotopic callosal inhibition. Brain and Language, 23(1), 116–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (1996). Hemispheric interactions in temporal judgments about spatially separated stimuli. Neuropsychology, 10(1), 42–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (1998). Sperry and the age of Aquarius: Science, values and the split brain. Neuropsychologia, 36(10), 1083–1087.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C., Boyd, L., Schulze, A., et al. (1998). Role of the commissures in interhemispheric temporal judgments. Neuropsychology, 12(4), 519–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crow, T. J., Crow, L. R., Done, D. J., et al. (1998). Relative hand skill predicts academic ability: Global deficits at the point of hemispheric indecision. Neuropsychologia, 36(12), 1275–1282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csermely, D. (2004). Lateralisation in birds of prey: Adaptive and phylogenetic considerations. Behavioural Processes, 67(3), 511–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, J. L. (1997). Neuropsychiatric manifestations of right hemisphere lesions. Brain and Language, 57(1), 22–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, J. (1997). Principles of psychopathology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Tranel, D., et al. (1996). A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature, 380(6574), 499–505 (see comments) (erratum appears in Nature, 381(6595), p. 810).

    Google Scholar 

  • Denenberg, V. H., Garbanati, J., Sherman, D. A., et al. (1978). Infantile stimulation induces brain lateralization in rats. Science, 201(4361), 1150–1152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dharmaretnam, M., & Andrew, R. J. (1994). Age-specific and stimulus-specific use of right and left eyes by the domestic chick. Animal Behaviour, 48(6), 1395–1406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dharmaretnam, M., & Rogers, L. J. (2005). Hemispheric specialization and dual processing in strongly versus weakly lateralized chicks. Behavioural Brain Research, 162(1), 62–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., Aarts, H., Bargh, J. A., et al. (2000). Past contact, stereotype strength, and automatic behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 531–544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drake, R. A., & Bingham, B. R. (1985). Induced lateral orientation and persuasibility. Brain and Cognition, 4(2), 156–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, C. S., Evans, L., & Marler, P. (1993). On the meaning of alarm calls – functional reference in an avian vocal system. Animal Behaviour, 46(1), 23–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evert, D. L., & Kmen, M. (2003). Hemispheric asymmetries for global and local processing as a function of stimulus exposure duration. Brain and Cognition, 51(1), 115–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eviatar, Z., & Just, M. A. (2006). Brain correlates of discourse processing: An fMRI investigation of irony and conventional metaphor comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 2348–2359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fabre-Thorpe, M., Fagot, J., Lorincz, E., et al. (1993). Laterality in cats: Paw preference and performance in a visuomotor activity. Cortex, 29(1), 15–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faust, M., & Mashal, N. (2007). The role of the right cerebral hemisphere in processing novel metaphoric expressions taken from poetry: A divided visual field study. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 860–870.

    Google Scholar 

  • Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). Right words and left words: Electrophysiological evidence for hemispheric differences in meaning processing. Cognitive Brain Research, 8(3), 373–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feinberg, T. E., Roane, D. M., Kwan, P. C., et al. (1994). Anosognosia and visuoverbal confabulation. Archives of Neurology, 51(5), 468–473.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández-Carriba, S., Loeches, A., Morcillo, A., et al. (2002). Asymmetry in facial expression of emotions by chimpanzees. Neuropsychologia, 40(9), 1523–1533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fink, G. R., Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., et al. (1999). Hemispheric asymmetries in global/local processing are modulated by perceptual salience. Neuropsychologia, 37(1), 31–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foldi, N. S. (1987). Appreciation of pragmatic interpretations of indirect commands: Comparison of right and left hemisphere brain-damaged patients. Brain and Language, 31(1), 88–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foldi, N. S., Cicone, M., & Gardner, H. (1983). Pragmatic aspects of communication in brain-damaged patients. In S. J. Segalowitz (Ed.), Language functions and brain organisation (pp. 51–86). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gainotti, G. (2002). The relationships between anatomical and cognitive locus of lesion in category-specific disorders. In E. M. E. Forde & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Category specificity in brain and mind (pp. 403–426). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1974). The shattered mind. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S., & LeDoux, J. E. (1978). The integrated mind. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gloning, I., Gloning, K., & Hoff, H. (1968). Neuropsychological symptoms and syndromes in lesions of the occipital lobe and the adjacent areas. Paris: Gauthier-Villars.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, E. (1990). Associative agnosias and the functions of the left hemisphere. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12(4), 467–484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, E., & Costa, L. D. (1981). Hemispheric differences in the acquisition and use of descriptive systems. Brain and Language, 14(1), 144–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, M. (1988). Drawing deficits in brain-damaged patients’ freehand pictures. Brain and Cognition, 8(2), 189–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Güntürkün, O., Diekamp, B., Manns, M., et al. (2000). Asymmetry pays: Visual lateralization improves discrimination success in pigeons. Current Biology, 10(17), 1079–1081.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haggard, M. P., & Parkinson, A. M. (1971). Stimulus and task factors as determinants of ear advantages. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23(2), 168–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, M. E., Güntürkün, O., Hopkins, W. D., et al. (2005). Lateralization of the vertebrate brain: Taking the side of model systems. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(45), 10351–10357.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hécaen, H., & Albert, M. L. (1978). Human neuropsychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., Scholes, R., & Watson, R. T. (1975). Auditory affective agnosia: Disturbed comprehension of affective speech. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 38(1), 69–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henson, R. N. A., Rugg, M. D., Shallice, T., et al. (2000). Confidence in recognition memory for words: Dissociating right prefrontal roles in episodic retrieval. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(6), 913–923.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, A. M., Robakiewicz, P. E., Tuttle, E. M., et al. (2006). Behavioural lateralisation in the Australian magpie (Gymnorhina tibicen). Laterality, 11(2), 110–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, W. D., & Marino, L. (2000). Asymmetries in cerebral width in nonhuman primate brains as revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Neuropsychologia, 38(4), 493–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Höppner, J., Kunesch, E., Buchmann, J., et al. (1999). Demyelination and axonal degeneration in corpus callosum assessed by analysis of transcallosally mediated inhibition in multiple sclerosis. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110(4), 748–756.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoptman, M. J., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). How and why do the two cerebral hemispheres interact? Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 195–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hough, M. S. (1990). Narrative comprehension in adults with right and left hemisphere brain-damage: Theme organization. Brain and Language, 38(2), 253–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivry, R. B., & Robertson, L. C. (1998). The two sides of perception. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jäncke, L., & Steinmetz, H. (2003). Anatomical brain asymmetries and their relevance for functional asymmetries. In K. Hugdahl & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The asymmetrical brain (pp. 187–230). Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, J. A., Brownell, H. H., Jacobs, J. R., et al. (1990). The effects of right hemisphere damage on the pragmatic interpretation of conversational remarks. Brain and Language, 38(2), 315–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, D. (1963). Right temporal-lobe damage: Perception of unfamiliar stimuli after damage. Archives of Neurology, 8, 264–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1988). Hemispheric interactions in depression. In Kinsbourne, M. (Ed.), Cerebral hemisphere function in depression (pp. 133–162). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1993). Orientational bias model of unilateral neglect: Evidence from attentional gradients within hemispace. In I. H. Robertson & J. C. Marshall (Eds.), Unilateral neglect: Clinical and experimental studies (pp. 63–86). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitterle, F. L., Christman, S., & Hellige, J. B. (1990). Hemispheric differences are found in the identification, but not the detection, of low versus high spatial frequencies. Perception and Psychophysics, 48(4), 297–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: A computational approach. Psychological Review, 94(2), 148–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landis, T., Cummings, J. L., Benson, D. F., et al. (1986). Loss of topographic familiarity: An environmental agnosia. Archives of Neurology, 43(2), 132–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippolis, G., Bisazza, A., Rogers, L. J., et al. (2002). Lateralisation of predator avoidance responses in three species of toads. Laterality, 7(2), 163–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lippolis, G., Westman, W., McAllan, B. M., et al. (2005). Lateralisation of escape responses in the striped-faced dunnart, Sminthopsis macroura (Dasyuridae: Marsupialia). Laterality, 10(5), 457–470.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. (1690). An essay on human understanding. London: Rivington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marsolek, C. J. (1995). Abstract visual-form representations in the left cerebral hemisphere. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perceptual Performance, 21(2), 375–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G., et al. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379(6566), 649–652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marzi, C. A., Bisiacchi, P., & Nicoletti, R. (1991). Is interhemispheric transfer of visuomotor information asymmetric? Evidence from a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 29(12), 1163–1177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mashal, N., & Faust, M. (2008). Right hemisphere sensitivity to novel metaphoric relations: Application of the signal detection theory. Brain and Language, 104(2), 103–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mashal, N., Faust, M., & Hendler, T. (2005). The role of the right hemisphere in processing nonsalient metaphorical meanings: Application of principal components analysis to fMRI data. Neuropsychologia, 43(14), 2084–2100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mashal, N., Faust, M., Hendler, T., et al. (2007). An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions. Brain and Language, 100(2), 115–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGilchrist, I. (2009). The master and his emissary: The divided brain and the making of the Western World. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, W. C., & Marchant, L. F. (1999). Laterality of hand use pays off in foraging success for wild chimpanzees. Primates, 40(3), 509–513.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Feinberg, T. E., et al. (2000). Anosognosia and asomatognosia during intracarotid amobarbital inactivation. Neurology, 55(6), 816–820.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, B.-U., Röricht, S., Gräfin von Einsiedel, H., et al. (1995). Inhibitory and excitatory interhemispheric transfers between motor cortical areas in normal subjects and patients with abnormalities of the corpus callosum. Brain, 118(2), 429–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel, E. M., & Troost, B. T. (1980). Palinopsia: Cerebral localization with CT. Neurology, 30(8), 887–889.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, L., & Rollman, G. B. (1980). Hemispheric asymmetry for auditory perception of temporal order. Neuropsychologia, 18(1), 41–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milo, R. G., & Quiatt, D. (1993). Glottogenesis and anatomically modern Homo sapiens: The evidence for and implications of a late origin of vocal language. Current Anthropology, 34(5), 569–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. J. (1998). A creative explosion? Theory of mind, language and the disembodied mind of the upper Palaeolithic. In S. J. Mithen (Ed.), Creativity in human evolution and prehistory (pp. 165–192). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. J. (2005). Singing Neanderthals: The origin of music, language, mind and body. London: Phoenix.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, T., Büttner, T., Kuhn, W., et al. (1995). Palinopsia as sensory epileptic phenomenon. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 91(6), 433–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mummery, C. J., Patterson, K., Hodges, J. R., et al. (1996). Generating “tiger” as an animal name or a word beginning with T: Differences in brain activation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 263(1373), 989–995.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagel, T. (1986). The view from nowhere. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nebes, R. D. (1974). Hemispheric specialization in commissurotomized man. Psychological Bulletin, 81(1), 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, M. E. R. (1994). Hemispheric asymmetries for temporal resolution: A signal detection analysis of threshold and bias. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 47(2), 291–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliveri, M., Rossini, P. M., Traversa, R., et al. (1999). Left frontal transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces contralesional extinction in patients with unilateral right brain damage. Brain, 122(9), 1731–1739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornstein, R., Herron, J., Johnstone, J., et al. (1979). Differential right hemisphere involvement in two reading tasks. Psychophysiology, 16(4), 398–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Bettinardi, V., et al. (1995). Different neural systems for the recognition of animals and man-made tools. NeuroReport, 6(12), 1637–1641.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1992). Hemispheric differences in mnemonic processing: The effects of left hemisphere interpretation. Neuropsychologia, 30(3), 293–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, C. J., & Friston, K. J. (2002). Functional imaging studies of category specificity. In E. M. E. Forde & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Category specificity in brain and mind (pp. 427–447). Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapp, A. M., Leube, D. T., Erb, M., et al. (2004). Neural correlates of metaphor processing. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 20(3), 395–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rausch, R. (1977). Cognitive strategies in patients with unilateral temporal lobe excisions. Neuropsychologia, 15(3), 385–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rausch, R. (1985). Differences in cognitive function with left and right temporal lobe dysfunction. In D. F. Benson & E. Zaidel (Eds.), The dual brain (pp. 247–261). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., et al. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661–670.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, L. C., Lamb, M. R., & Knight, R. T. (1988). Effects of lesions of temporal-parietal junction on perceptual and attentional processing in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 8(10), 3757–3769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson, L. C., & Lamb, M. R. (1991). Neuropsychological contributions to theories of part/whole organisation. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 299–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robins, A., & Rogers, L. J. (2006). Complementary and lateralized forms of processing in Bufo marinus for novel and familiar prey. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 86(2), 214–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, L. J. (2000). Evolution of hemisphere specialisation: Advantages and disadvantages. Brain and Language, 73(2), 236–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, L. J. (2005). Cognitive and social advantages of having a lateralized brain. In Y. B. Malashichev & A. W. Deckel (Eds.), Behavioral and morphological asymmetries in vertebrates (pp. 129–139). Austin: Landes Bioscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, L. J., & Kaplan, G. (2006). An eye for a predator: Lateralization in birds, with particular reference to the Australian magpie. In Y. B. Malashichev & A. W. Deckel (Eds.), Behavioral and morphological asymmetries in vertebrates (pp. 47–57). Austin: Landes Bioscience.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, L. J., & Workman, L. (1989). Light exposure during incubation affects competitive behaviour in domestic chicks. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 23, 187–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, L. J., Zucca, P., & Vallortigara, G. (2004). Advantages of having a lateralized brain. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl. 6), S420–S422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Röricht, S., Irlbacher, K., Petrow, E., et al. (1997). Normwerte transkallosal und kortikospinal vermittelter Effekte einer hemisphärenselektiven magnetischen Kortexreizung beim Menschen. Zeitschrift für Elektroenzephalographie, Elektromyographie und Verwandte Gebiete, 28, 34–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, E. D., Thompson, R. D., & Yenkosky, J. (1977). Lateralisation of affective prosody in the brain and callosal integration of hemispheric language functions. Brain and Language, 56(1), 27–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saron, C. D., & Davidson, R. J. (1989). Visual evoked potential measures of interhemispheric transfer time in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 103(5), 1115–1138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saron, C. D., Foxe, J. J., Simpson, G. V., et al. (2002). Interhemispheric visuomotor activation: Spatiotemporal electrophysiology related to reaction time. In E. Zaidel & M. Iacoboni (Eds.), The parallel brain: The cognitive neuroscience of the corpus callosum (pp. 171–219). Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saron, C. D., Foxe, J. J., Schroeder, C. E., et al. (2003). Complexities of interhemispheric communication in sensorimotor tasks revealed by high-density event-related potential mapping. In K. Hugdahl & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The asymmetrical brain (pp. 341–408). Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, G. L., DeBuse, C. J., & Seger, C. A. (2007). Right hemisphere metaphor processing? Characterizing the lateralization of semantic processes. Brain and Language, 100(2), 127–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneiderman, E. I., Murasugi, K. G., & Saddy, J. D. (1992). Story arrangement ability in right-brain damaged patients. Brain and Language, 43(1), 107–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnider, A., Gutbrod, K., Hess, C. W., et al. (1996). Memory without context: Amnesia with confabulations after infarction of the right capsular genu. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 61(2), 186–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergent, J. (1982). The cerebral balance of power: Confrontation or cooperation? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8(2), 253–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starkstein, S. E., Fedoroff, J. P., Price, T. R., et al. (1992). Anosognosia in patients with cerebrovascular lesions: A study of causative factors. Stroke, 23(10), 1446–1453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stringaris, A. K., Medford, N. C., Giampietro, V., et al. (2007). Deriving meaning: Distinct neural mechanisms for metaphoric, literal, and non-meaningful sentences. Brain and Language, 100(2), 150–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stump, E. (2009). The problem of evil: Analytic philosophy and narrative. In O. D. Crisp & M. C. Rea (Eds.), Analytic theology: New essays in the philosophy of theology (pp. 251–264). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T. (1991). Disturbance of self-awareness after frontal system damage. In G. P. Prigatano & D. L. Schacter (Eds.), Awareness of deficit after brain injury: Clinical and theoretical issues (pp. 66–83). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swisher, L., & Hirsh, I. J. (1972). Brain damage and the ordering of two temporally successive stimuli. Neuropsychologia, 10(2), 137–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tootell, R. B., Mendola, J. D., Hadjikhani, N. K., et al. (1998). The representation of the ipsilateral visual field in human cerebral cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 95(3), 818–824.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallortigara, G. (1992). Right hemisphere advantage for social recognition in the chick. Neuropsychologia, 30(9), 761–768.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallortigara, G., Rogers, L. J., Bisazza, A., et al. (1998). Complementary right and left hemifield use for predatory and agonistic behaviour in toads. NeuroReport, 9(14), 3341–3344.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Kleeck, M. H. (1989). Hemispheric differences in global versus local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli by normal subjects: New data and a meta-analysis of previous studies. Neuropsychologia, 27(9), 1165–1178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ventolini, N., Ferrero, E. A., Sponza, S., et al. (2005). Laterality in the wild: Preferential hemifield use during predatory and sexual behaviour in the black-winged stilt (Himantopus himantopus). Animal Behaviour, 69, 1077–1084.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogeley, K., Bussfeld, P., Newen, A., et al. (2001). Mind reading: Neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self-perspective. NeuroImage, 14(1, Pt 1), 170–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warrington, E. K., & McCarthy, R. A. (1987). Categories of knowledge: Further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110(5), 1273–1296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wymer, J. H., Lindman, L. S., & Booksh, R. L. (2002). A neuropsychological perspective of aprosody: Features, function, assessment, and treatment. Applied Neuropsychology, 9(1), 37–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoshida, T., Yoshino, A., Takahashi, Y., et al. (2007). Comparison of hemispheric asymmetry in global and local information processing and interference in divided and selective attention using spatial frequency filters. Experimental Brain Research, 181(3), 519–529.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Iain McGilchrist .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this entry

Cite this entry

McGilchrist, I. (2015). Divine Understanding and the Divided Brain. In: Clausen, J., Levy, N. (eds) Handbook of Neuroethics. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4707-4_99

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4707-4_99

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-007-4706-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-007-4707-4

  • eBook Packages: Humanities, Social Sciences and Law

Publish with us

Policies and ethics