Skip to main content

Comprehending the Neurological Substratum of Paraverbal Communications: The Invention of SplitSpec Technology

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of Neurosociology

Abstract

An earlier book chapter (Gregory 1999) reviewed the specific background of our investigations into the social significance and neurology of the lower speech frequency in human communication. This lower speech frequency, termed the paraverbal frequency (beneath.5 kHz), was found to convey important nonverbal social information, and the previously published chapter outlined the methodology and results of numerous experiments showing how the paraverbal signal differs from the verbal; specifically, how it acts as an elemental mechanism of social status accommodation and social convergence between conversation partners. The present chapter will continue from where the last chapter left off and aims first to merge our past results and observations with a theoretical account making use of pertinent findings from physical anthropology, cognitive psychology, and neurology. With the theoretical background established, we will then review how our research led to the development of a technological innovation called “SplitSpec Technology,” and how this innovation will fit into the future of human electronic communications.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Professor Hans Thewissen is professor of anatomy at the Northeastern Ohio Universities College of Medicine, and his specialized research interest is in marine mammals. The lateral lines in fish have a precursor inserted within the aural system of mammals – the lateral lines are spiraled and condensed thus forming the mammalian cochlea (Partridge 1982). The contralateral reflex, discussed above, is called C-start in fishes reflecting the “C” shape of the fish after the defensive reflex is affected. A marine, snake predator has made use of this reflex in pursuing its prey by first circling the fish, then activating the fish’s C-start reflex with a slight aquatic perturbation in such a way to cause the fish to directly swim into the snake’s mouth, thus turning the fish’s defensive strategy into the snake’s offensive strategy http://www.livescience.com/5503-snake-tricks-fish-swim-mouth.htlm.

  2. 2.

    This difference in neural wiring has undoubtedly developed owing to the major difference in somatic position of (1) the eyes perceiving an apportioned but partially shared field of view as mimicked by the neural function of the optic chiasm, and (2) the ears perceiving a shared ambient air space but an apportioned proximity of aural space as mimicked by the neural function of the aural pathways (Geffen et al. 1971).

  3. 3.

    This left hemisphere lateralization for conspecific vocalizations has been found in Japanese macaques (Zoloth et al. 1979; May et al. 1988) as well as rhesus monkeys (Hauser et al. 1998).

  4. 4.

    Though former Vice President Gore won the popular vote, he did not become president as the Supreme Court judgment caused George W. Bush to take the office of president.

  5. 5.

    The aural system in this study used the same headset microphone system as in previous research projects, and the visual system made use of closed circuit television monitors with cameras installed so participants could interact visually with one another.

  6. 6.

    All prospective participants were administered the Oldfield Handedness Inventory (1970). Only right-handed participants could participate in this research, as there is a higher probability that left-handers may have reversed cerebral lateralization than right- handed participants. This practice was done in order to prevent a possible confound, and is generally the practice in dichotic listening research.

  7. 7.

    No significant differences were found between the task completion time means for the confounded and control conditions. This was an unanticipated finding and we offer a possible explanation for this result. Though our theory establishes that routing the signal to the appropriate cerebral location enhances timely performance, the dichotic, contralateral, routing manipulation of the high and low-pass signals to the inappropriate hemispheres may involve a neuronal high/low-pass packaging of individual frequency bands, making them more efficient for rerouting than the raw, unfiltered monaural control signal.

  8. 8.

    This result may be caused by a similar neural packaging affect as noted in note 7 above.

  9. 9.

    The driving simulator (Simulator Systems International S-3300 Modular Driving simulator) consisted of simulated driving equipment, i.e. wheel, pedals, etc., including consoles with the necessary audio/visual effects.

References

  • Bradshaw, J. L., & Nettleton, N. C. (1983). Human cerebral asymmetry. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, C. (1957). On human communication. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curry, F. K. (1967). A comparison of left-handed and right-handed subjects on verbal and non-verbal dichotic listening tasks. Cortex, 3, 343–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (1970). The bisected brain. New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (1985). The social brain. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication – Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain, 123, 1293–1326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geffen, G., Bradshaw, J. L., & Wallace, G. (1971). Interhemispheric effects on reaction time to verbal and nonverbal visual stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 415–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H., & Coupland, N. (1991). Language: Contexts and consequences. Pacific Grove: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H., Smith, P. M., & Patricia, M. (1979). Accommodation theory: Optimal levels of convergence. In G. Howard, N. Robert, & R. St. Clair (Eds.), Language and social psychology (pp. 45–65). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H., Mulac, A., Bradac, J. J., & Johnson, P. (1987). Speech accommodation theory: The next decade and beyond. In Communication yearbook (Vol. 10, pp. 13–48). Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. W., Jr. (1994). Sounds of power and deference: Acoustic analysis of macro social constraints on micro interaction. Sociological Perspectives, 37, 497–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. W., Jr. (1999). Navigating the sound stream of human social interaction. In D. D. Franks & T. S. Smith (Eds.), Mind, brain, and society (pp. 247–285). Stamford: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. W., Jr., & Gallagher, T. J. (2002). Spectral analysis of candidates’ nonverbal vocal communication: Predicting U.S. Presidential election outcomes. Social Psychology Quarterly, 65, 298–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. W., Jr., & Webster, S. (1996). A nonverbal signal in voices of interview partners effectively predicts communication accommodation and social status perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1231–1240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. W., Jr., Webster, S. W., & Huang, G. (1993). Voice pitch and amplitude convergence as a metric of quality in dyadic interviews. Language and Communication, 13, 195–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. W., Jr., Dagan, K. A., & Webster, S. (1997). Evaluating the relation of vocal accommodation in conversation partners’ fundamental frequencies to perceptions of communication quality. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 23–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. W., Jr., Green, B. E., Carrothers, R. M., Dagan, K. A., & Webster, S. W. (2001). Verifying the primary of voice fundamental frequency in social status accommodation. Language and Communication, 21, 37–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, S. W., Jr., Kalkhoff, W., Harkness, S. K., & Paull, J. L. (2009). Targeted high and low speech frequency bands to right and left ears respectively improve task performance and perceived sociability in dyadic conversations. Laterality: Asymetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 14, 423–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Agnetta, B., & Perez, C. (1998). Orienting asymmetries in rhesus monkeys: The effects of time domain changes on acoustic perception. Animal Behavior, 56, 41–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heise, D. R. (1970). The semantic differential and attitude research. In G. F. Summers (Ed.), Attitude measurement. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewes, G. W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Current Anthropology, 14, 5–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kalkhoff, W., & Gregory, S. W., Jr. (2008). Beyond the issues: Nonverbal vocal communication, power rituals, and ‘Rope-A-Dopes’ in the 2008 presidential debates. Current Research in Social Psychology, 14, 39–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalkhoff, W., Gregory, S. W., Jr., & Melamed, D. (2009). Effects of dichotically enhanced electronic communication on crash risk and performance during simulated driving. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 108, 449–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, D. (1961). Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 166–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, D. (1967). Functional asymmetry of the brain in dichotic listening. Cortex, 3, 163–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, D. (1973a). The asymmetry of the human brain. Scientific American, 228, 70–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, D. (1973b). Manual activity during speaking. I. Right handers. Neuropsychologia, 11, 45–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer, E. (1964). Elimination of verbal cues in judgments of emotion from voice. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 390–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (1975). On the origins of language: An introduction to the evolution of human speech. New York: Macmillan Publishing Co., Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, B., Moody, D. B., & Stebbins, W. C. (1988). The significant features of Japanese macaque Coo sounds. Animal Behavior, 36, 1432–1444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McManus, C. (2002). Right hand, left hand: The origins of asymmetry in brains, bodies, atoms and cultures. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. C. (1977). Handbook of research design and social measurement. New York: Mckay.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milner, B., Taylor, L., & Sperry, R. W. (1968). Lateralized suppression of dichotically presented digits after commissural section in man. Science, 161, 184–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ng, S. H., & Bradac, J. J. (1993). Power in language: Verbal communication and social influence. Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1970). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. (1964). Semantic differential technique in the comparative study of cultures. American Anthropologist, 66, 171–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Partridge, B. L. (1982). The structure and function of fish schools. Scientific American, 246, 114–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. L. (1995). A parallel process model of nonverbal communication. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 19, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, M. R. (1951). Representations of the two ears at the auditory cortex. American Journal of Physiology, 167, 147–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, E. E. (1971). Social psychology and contemporary society. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarles, H. B. (1975). A human ethological approach to communication: Ideas in transit around the Cartesian impasse. In A. Kendon, R. M. Harris, & M. R. Key (Eds.), Organization of behavior in face-to- face interaction. The Hague: Mouton Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shubin, N. (2008). Your inner fish: A journey into the 3.5 billion history of the human body. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry, R. W. (1959). The growth of nerve circuits. Scientific American, 201, 68–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Springer, S. P., & Deutsch, G. (1981). Left brain right brain. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starkweather, J. A. (1956a). Content-free speech as a source of information about the speaker. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 394–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starkweather, J. A. (1956b). The communication value of content-free speech. The American Journal of Psychology, 69, 121–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zoloth, S. R., Petersen, M. R., Beecher, M. D., Green, S., Marler, P., Moody, D. B., & Stebbins, W. (1979). Species-specific perceptual processing of vocal sounds by monkeys. Science, 204, 870–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stanford W. Gregory Jr. Ph.D. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gregory, S.W., Kalkhoff, W. (2013). Comprehending the Neurological Substratum of Paraverbal Communications: The Invention of SplitSpec Technology. In: Franks, D.D., Turner, J.H. (eds) Handbook of Neurosociology. Handbooks of Sociology and Social Research. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4473-8_24

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics