Skip to main content

The Role of the Observer in Goal-Directed Behavior

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Wandering Towards a Goal

Part of the book series: The Frontiers Collection ((FRONTCOLL))

  • 991 Accesses

Abstract

Systems producing goal-oriented behavior manage to approach the desired goal from multiple initial states, circumventing the obstacles they meet along the way. The mathematical description of this type of evolution is formulated in terms of an open subsystem whose entropy decreases in time. Yet ultimately, the laws of physics are reversible, so entropy variations are necessarily a consequence of the level of description the observer has chosen to employ. In order to reconcile different levels of descriptions, systems yielding goal-directed behavior must transfer the information about initial conditions and about circumvented obstacles to other degrees of freedom outside their own boundaries. To operate steadily, they must consume ordered degrees of freedom provided as input, and be dispensed of disordered outputs that act as wastes from the point of view of the aimed objective. In short, goal-oriented behavior requires metabolism, even if conducted by non-living agents. Here I argue that for a physical system to display goal-directed behavior, its borders must be carefully tailored so as to entail the appropriate information balance sheet. In this game, observers play the role of tailors: They design agents by setting the limits of the system of interest. Goal-directed behavior hence only exists in the eye of the beholder. Brain-guided subjects perform this creative observation task naturally, implying that the observation of goal-oriented behavior is a goal-oriented behavior on its own. Minds evolved to cut out pieces of reality and endow them with intentionality, because ascribing intentionality is an efficient way of modeling the world, and making predictions. One most remarkable agent of whom we have first-hand evidence of its goal-pursuing attitude is our own self. Notably, this agent is simultaneously the subject and the object of observation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 64.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 89.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  1. Bennett, C.H.: The thermodynamics of computation-a review. Int. J. Theoret. Phys. 21(12), 905–940 (1982)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Damasio, A.R.: The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. Harcourt Brace & Co, San Diego (1999)

    Google Scholar 

  3. Dennet, D.C.: The Intentional Stance. MIT Press, Cambridge (1989)

    Google Scholar 

  4. Dennet, D.C.: Consciousness explained. Little, Brown and Co, Boston (1991)

    Google Scholar 

  5. Dennet, D.C.: Darwin’s dangerous idea: evolution and the meaning of life. Simon & Schuster, New York (1995)

    Google Scholar 

  6. Fredkin, E., Toffoli, T.: Conservative logic. Int. J. Theoret. Phys. 21(3–4), 219–253 (1982)

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  7. Hofstadter, D.: I Am a Strange Loop. Basic Books, New York (2007)

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  8. Kauffman, S.A.: The Origins of Order: Self Organization and Selection in Evolution. Oxford University Press, New York (1993)

    Google Scholar 

  9. Landauer, R.W.: Irreversibility and heat generation in the computing process IBM. J. Res. Dev. 5(3), 183–191 (1961)

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  10. MacKay, D.: Information Theory, Inference and Learning Algorithms. Cambridge University Press, New York (2003)

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  11. Maxwell, J.C.: Theory of heat. Longmans, Green, London, New York (1908)

    Google Scholar 

  12. Minsky, M.: Society of Mind. Simon and Schuster, New York (1986)

    Google Scholar 

  13. Norretranders, T.: The User Illusion: Cutting Consciousness Down to Size. Viking, New York (1999)

    Google Scholar 

  14. Parrondo, J.M.R., Horowitz, J.M., Sagawa, T.: Thermodynamics of information. Nat. Phys. 11, 131–139 (2015)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Perunov, N., Marsland, R.A., England, J.L.: Statistical physics of adaptation. Phys. Rev. X 6(2), 021036 (2016). 12 pages

    Google Scholar 

  16. Shannon, C.E.: A mathematical theory of communication. Bell Syst. Tech. J. 27(3), 379–423 (1948)

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  17. Tononi, G.: Consciousness as integrated information: a provisional manifesto. Biol. Bull. 215, 216–242 (2008)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Toyabe, S., Sagawa, T., Ueda, M., Muneyuki, E., Sano, M.: Experimental demonstration of information-to-energy conversion and validation of the generalized Jarzynski equality. Nat. Phys. 6, 988–992 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Inés Samengo .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Samengo, I. (2018). The Role of the Observer in Goal-Directed Behavior. In: Aguirre, A., Foster, B., Merali, Z. (eds) Wandering Towards a Goal. The Frontiers Collection. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-75726-1_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics