Skip to main content

Learning Morphological Constructions

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Construction of Words

Part of the book series: Studies in Morphology ((SUMO,volume 4))

Abstract

The great variability of morphological structure across languages makes it uncontroversial that morphology is learned. Yet, morphology presents formidable learning challenges, on par with those of syntax. This article takes a constructionist perspective in assuming that morphological constructions are a major outcome of the learning process. However, the existence of morphological paradigms in many languages suggests that they are often not the only outcome. The article reviews domain-general approaches to achieving this outcome. The primary focus is on mechanisms proposed within the associative/connectionist tradition, which are compared with Bayesian approaches. The issues discussed include the role of prediction and prediction error in learning, generative vs. discriminative learning models, directionality of associations, the roles of (unexpectedly) present vs. absent stimuli, general-to-specific vs. specific-to-general learning, and the roles of type and token frequency. In the process, the notion of a construction itself is shown to be more complicated that it first appears.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Bayesian models make a similar prediction, except that competition occurs between inferred causes rather than predictive cues.

  2. 2.

    This result suggests that the difference between child and adult learners in Ramscar et al. (2013b) may be quantitative rather than qualitative in nature: children may be habituated by repetition more quickly than adults are, and therefore shift attention away from repeated stimuli more quickly than an adult would.

  3. 3.

    p(form|meaning) = p(form,meaning)/p(meaning), where p(form,meaning) is frequency of the form-meaning mapping and p(meaning) is the sum of these frequencies across all forms sharing a meaning. Assuming that forms sharing a meaning compete with each other for selection, with the outcome of this competition determined by relative schema strength, schema strength could be proportional to either frequency of the schema or the conditional probability of form given meaning without any consequences for the outcome of the competition.

  4. 4.

    One may also consider updating blig- in the opposite, non-normative direction based on the fact that unused connections are pruned.

  5. 5.

    This point is traditionally emphasized by error-driven models of learning, where unexpected means predicted but not observed. This point may therefore appear to be inconsistent with the Hebbian approach taken to modeling the data below, which does not rely on prediction. However, even in the Hebbian framework, in order to notice the absence of something, one needs to have experienced it. Otherwise, there is no representation for the absent stimulus in the learner’s mind.

  6. 6.

    Instead, unfaithful mappings might benefit from encountering the outputs of such mappings as a block at an early point during the experiment (a la Carvalho and Goldstone 2016), when the learner’s impression of what plural forms are like is still easily malleable.

  7. 7.

    With respect to form-meaning mappings, language calls for bidirectionality because a listener who encounters a form and links it to a meaning wishes to be able to reproduce what she just heard when expressing the same meaning. Similarly, when acquiring a morphological paradigm, one wishes to be able to fill in any form in a paradigm given any other form (Ackerman and Malouf 2013). Unfortunately, we know very little about directionality in paradigm learning (though see Jun and Albright 2017, for first steps in addressing this question).

References

  • Ackerman, F., and R. Malouf. 2013. Morphological organization: The low conditional entropy conjecture. Language 89 (3): 429–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albright, A., and B. Hayes. 2003. Rules vs. analogy in English past tenses: A computational/experimental study. Cognition 90 (2): 119–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ambridge, B., and E.V. Lieven. 2011. Child language acquisition: Contrasting theoretical approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ambridge, B., J.M. Pine, C.F. Rowland, and C.R. Young. 2008. The effect of verb semantic class and verb frequency (entrenchment) on children’s and adults’ graded judgments of argument-structure overgeneralisation errors. Cognition 106 (1): 87–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ambridge, B., J.M. Pine, C.F. Rowland, and F. Chang. 2012. The roles of verb semantics, entrenchment and morphophonology in the retreat from dative argument structure overgeneralization errors. Language 88 (1): 45–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arcediano, F., M. Escobar, and R.R. Miller. 2003. Temporal integration and temporal backward associations in human and nonhuman subjects. Animal Learning & Behavior 31 (3): 242–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2005. Bidirectional associations in humans and rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 31 (3): 301–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S.E., and S.M. Ebenholtz. 1962. The principle of associative symmetry. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 106 (2): 135–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, M. 2016. Competition and the lexicon. In Livelli di Analisi e fenomeni di interfaccia. Atti del XLVII Congresso Internazionale della Società di Linguistica Italiana, ed. A. Elia, C. Iacobini, and M. Voghera, 39–52. Roma: Bulzoni Editore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, F.G., J.M. Ennis, and B.J. Spiering. 2007. A neurobiological theory of automaticity in perceptual categorization. Psychological Review 114: 632–656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R.H. 1992. Quantitative aspects of morphological productivity. In Yearbook of morphology 1991, ed. G. Booij and J. van Marle, 109–149. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R.H., T. Dijkstra, and R. Schreuder. 1997. Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model. Journal of Memory and Language 37 (1): 94–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R.H., P. Milin, D.F. Đurđević, P. Hendrix, and M. Marelli. 2011. An amorphous model for morphological processing in visual comprehension based on naive discriminative learning. Psychological Review 118 (3): 438–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barðdal, J. 2008. Productivity: Evidence from case and argument structure in Icelandic. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baroni, M., J. Matiasek, and H. Trost. 2002. Unsupervised discovery of morphologically related words based on orthographic and semantic similarity. In Proceedings of the workshop on Morphological and Phonological Learning of ACL/SIGPHON-2002, 48–57. Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, M., and M. Gouskova. 2016. Source-oriented generalizations as grammar inference in Russian vowel deletion. Linguistic Inquiry 47 (3): 391–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G. 2010. Construction morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boudelaa, S., and M.G. Gaskell. 2002. A re-examination of the default system for Arabic plurals. Language & Cognitive Processes 17 (3): 321–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, J.K., and A.E. Goldberg. 2011. Learning what not to say: The role of statistical preemption and categorization in a-adjective production. Language 87 (1): 55–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D. 1963. The ontogeny of English phrase structure. Language 39: 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M.D., R.E. Brody, P.J. Brooks, V. Sudhalter, J.A. Ross, L. Catalano, and S.M. Fisch. 1990. Exploring language acquisition in children with a miniature artificial language: Effects of item and pattern frequency, arbitrary subclasses, and correction. Journal of Memory & Language 29 (5): 591–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, P.J., M.D.S. Braine, L. Catalano, R.E. Brody, and V. Sudhalter. 1993. Acquisition of gender-like noun subclasses in an artificial language: The contribution of phonological markers to learning. Journal of Memory & Language 32: 79–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buz, E., M.K. Tanenhaus, and T.F. Jaeger. 2016. Dynamically adapted context-specific hyper-articulation: Feedback from interlocutors affects speakers’ subsequent pronunciations. Journal of Memory and Language 89: 68–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J. 1985. Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2001. Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2003. Cognitive processes in grammaticalization. In The new psychology of language, ed. M. Tomasello, vol. 2, 145–167. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caplan, Jeremy B., Kathy L. Boulton, and Christina L. Gagné. 2014. Associative asymmetry of compound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 40: 1163–1171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappelle, B. 2006. Particle placement and the case for “allostructions”. Constructions SV1(7): 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carvalho, P.F., and R.L. Goldstone. 2016. The encoding of characteristic and diagnostic properties during interleaved and blocked category learning. Paper presented at the 57th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Boston, MA, November 17–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. 1981. Principles and parameters in syntactic theory. In Explanation in linguistics: The logical problem of language acquisition, ed. N. Hornstein and D. Lightfoot, 32–75. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clahsen, H. 1999. Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (6): 991–1013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A.L., R.M. Nosofsky, and S.R. Zaki. 2001. Category variability, exemplar similarity, and perceptual classification. Memory & Cognition 29 (8): 1165–1175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croft, W. 2001. Radical construction grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dąbrowska, E. 2004. Language, mind and brain: Some psychological and neurological constraints on theories of grammar. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2012. Different speakers, different grammars: Individual differences in native language attainment. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2 (3): 219–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J., and T. Beckers. 2003. Secondary task difficulty modulates forward blocking in human contingency learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 56B (4): 345–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N.C. 2006. Language acquisition as rational contingency learning. Applied Linguistics 27 (1): 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ervin, S.M. 1961. Changes with age in the verbal determinants of word-association. American Journal of Psychology 74: 361–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fellbaum, C. 1996. Co-occurrence and antonymy. International Journal of Lexicography 8 (4): 281–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C.J., P. Kay, and M.C. O'Connor. 1988. Regularity and idiomaticity in grammatical constructions: The case of let alone. Language 64 (3): 501–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frigo, L., and J.L. McDonald. 1998. Properties of phonological markers that affect the acquisition of gender-like subclasses. Journal of Memory and Language 39 (2): 218–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gershkoff-Stowe, L., and L.B. Smith. 1997. A curvilinear trend in naming errors as a function of early vocabulary growth. Cognitive Psychology 34 (1): 37–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A.E. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2002. Surface generalizations: An alternative to alternations. Cognitive Linguistics 13 (4): 327–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2006. Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A.E., D.M. Casenhiser, and N. Sethuraman. 2004. Learning argument structure generalizations. Cognitive Linguistics 15: 289–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M.H., and J.A. Schwade. 2010. From birds to words: Perception of structure in social interactions guides vocal development and language learning. In The Oxford handbook of developmental behavioral neuroscience, ed. M.S. Blumberg, J.H. Freeman, and S.R. Robinson, 708–729. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, R.L. 2002. Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science 13 (5): 431–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, M., J.L. Elman, and K.G. Daugherty. 1995. Default generalisation in connectionist networks. Language and Cognitive Processes 10 (6): 601–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon, Z., and V. Kapatsinski. 2017. Putting old tools to new uses: The role of form accessibility in semantic extension. Cognitive Psychology 98: 22–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J. 2001. Lexical frequency in morphology: Is everything relative? Linguistics 39 (6): 1041–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2003. Causes and consequences of word structure. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, B. 2004. Phonological acquisition in optimality theory: The early stages. In Constraints in phonological acquisition, ed. R. Kager, J. Pater, and W. Zonneveld, 158–203. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D.O. 1949. The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological approach. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilpert, M. 2008. Germanic future constructions: A usage-based approach to language change. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Horwood, G. 2001. Anti-faithfulness and subtractive morphology. Ms., Rutgers University, available as ROA, 466-0901.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, A., and T.L. Griffiths. 2009. Differential use of implicit negative evidence in generative and discriminative language learning. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 22: 754–762.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, A.S., and T.E. Griffiths. 2010. Effects of generative and discriminative learning on use of category variability. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society 32: 242–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, S., C. Paradis, M.L. Murphy, and C. Willners. 2007. Googling for ‘opposites’: A web-based study of antonym canonicity. Corpora 2 (2): 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jun, J., and A. Albright. 2017. Speakers’ knowledge of alternations is asymmetrical: Evidence from Seoul Korean verb paradigms. Journal of Linguistics 53: 567–611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Justeson, J.S., and S.M. Katz. 1991. Co-occurrences of antonymous adjectives and their contexts. Computational Linguistics 17: 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahana, M.J. 2002. Associative symmetry and memory theory. Memory & Cognition 30: 823–840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamin, L.J. 1969. Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In Punishment and aversive behavior, ed. B.A. Campbell and R.M. Church, 279–296. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kapatsinski, V. 2005. Productivity of Russian stem extensions: Evidence for and a formalization of network theory. MA thesis, The University of New Mexico.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2013. Conspiring to mean: Experimental and computational evidence for a usage-based harmonic approach to morphophonology. Language 89 (1): 110–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2017. Learning a subtractive morphological system: Statistics and representations. Proceedings of the Boston University Conference on Language Development 41 (1): 357–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. Forthcoming. Changing minds changing tools: From learning theory to language acquisition to language change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kapatsinski, V., and Z. Harmon. 2017. A Hebbian account of entrenchment and (over-)extension in language learning. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society 39: 2366–2371.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S., H. Cornish, and K. Smith. 2008. Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105 (31): 10681–10686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köpcke, K.-M. 1998. The acquisition of plural marking in English and German revisited: Schemata versus rules. Journal of Child Language 25 (2): 293–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köpcke, K.-M., and V. Wecker. 2017. Source-and product-oriented strategies in L2 acquisition of plural marking in German. Morphology 27: 77–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruschke, J.K. 2006. Locally Bayesian learning with applications to retrospective revaluation and highlighting. Psychological Review 113 (4): 677–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurisu, K. 2001. The phonology of morpheme realization. Doctoral dissertation, UC Santa Cruz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T.K., and S.T. Dumais. 1997. A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review 104 (2): 211–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar, Theoretical prerequisites. Vol. 1. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Łubowicz, A. 2007. Paradigmatic contrast in polish. Journal of Slavic Linguistics 15: 229–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N.J. 1975. A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review 82 (4): 276–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madlener, K. 2016. Input optimization: Effects of type and token frequency manipulations in instructed second language learning. In Experience counts: Frequency effects in language, ed. H. Behrens and S. Pfänder, 133–174. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maniwa, K., A. Jongman, and T. Wade. 2009. Acoustic characteristics of clearly spoken English fricatives. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 125 (6): 3962–3973.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, J.J. 1998. Morpheme structure constraints and paradigm occultation. Chicago Linguistic Society 32 (2): 123–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L. 2001. Failures to learn and their remediation: A Hebbian account. In Mechanisms of cognitive development: Behavioral and neural perspectives, ed. J.L. McClelland and R.S. Siegler, 97–121. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J.L., B.L. McNaughton, and R.C. O'Reilly. 1995. Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review 102 (3): 419–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKenzie, C.R., and L.A. Mikkelsen. 2007. A Bayesian view of covariation assessment. Cognitive Psychology 54 (1): 33–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McMurray, B., J.S. Horst, and L.K. Samuelson. 2012. Word learning emerges from the interaction of online referent selection and slow associative learning. Psychological Review 119 (4): 831–877.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. 1963. The origin of associations within the same grammatical class. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 2 (3): 250–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 1966. A study of word association. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 5: 548–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, M.L. 2006. Antonyms as lexical constructions: Or, why paradigmatic construction is not an oxymoron. Constructions SV1(8): 1–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naigles, L.G., and S.A. Gelman. 1995. Overextensions in comprehension and production revisited: Preferential-looking in a study of dog, cat, and cow. Journal of Child Language 22 (1): 19–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nesset, T. 2008. Abstract phonology in a concrete model. Cognitive linguistics and the morphology-phonology interface. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nosofsky, R.M. 1988. Similarity, frequency, and category representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 14 (1): 54–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Donnell, T.J. 2015. Productivity and reuse in language: A theory of linguistic computation and storage. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oldfield, R.C., and A. Wingfield. 1965. Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 17: 273–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I.P. 1927. Conditioned reflexes. An nvestigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Mineola: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pereira, A.F., L.B. Smith, and C. Yu. 2014. A bottom-up view of toddler word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21 (1): 178–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfors, A., K. Ransom, and D.J. Navarro. 2014. People ignore token frequency when deciding how widely to generalize. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society 36: 2759–2764.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piantadosi, S.T., H. Tily, and E. Gibson. 2012. The communicative function of ambiguity in language. Cognition 122 (3): 280–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S., and A. Prince. 1988. On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition 28 (1): 73–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poser, W.J. 1990. Evidence for foot structure in Japanese. Language 66 (1): 78–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramscar, M., and D. Yarlett. 2007. Linguistic self-correction in the absence of feedback: A new approach to the logical problem of language acquisition. Cognitive Science 31 (6): 927–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramscar, M., D. Yarlett, M. Dye, K. Denny, and K. Thorpe. 2010. The effects of feature-label-order and their implications for symbolic learning. Cognitive Science 34 (6): 909–957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramscar, M., M. Dye, and J. Klein. 2013a. Children value informativity over logic in word learning. Psychological Science 24 (6): 1017–1023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramscar, M., M. Dye, and S.M. McCauley. 2013b. Error and expectation in language learning: The curious absence of mouses in adult speech. Language 89 (4): 760–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Regier, T., and S. Gahl. 2004. Learning the unlearnable: The role of missing evidence. Cognition 93 (2): 147–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A., and A.R. Wagner. 1972. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory, ed. A.H. Black and W.F. Prokasy, 64–99. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, T.T., and J.L. McClelland. 2004. Semantic cognition: A parallel distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schertz, J. 2013. Exaggeration of featural contrasts in clarifications of misheard speech in English. Journal of Phonetics 41 (3): 249–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth, S., E. Buz, and T.F. Jaeger. 2016. Dynamic hyperarticulation of coda voicing contrasts. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 139 (2): EL31–EL37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smolek, A., and V. Kapatsinski. in preparation. The importance of contiguity for learning paradigmatic mappings. Ms., U Oregon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, D., and J.L. McClelland. 2009. When should we expect indirect effects in human contingency learning. Proceedings of the Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society 31: 206–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suttle, L., and A.E. Goldberg. 2011. The partial productivity of constructions as induction. Linguistics 49 (6): 1237–1269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taatgen, N.A., and J.R. Anderson. 2002. Why do children learn to say “broke”? A model of learning the past tense without feedback. Cognition 86 (2): 123–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tassoni, C.J. 1995. The least mean squares network with information coding: A model of cue learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 21 (1): 193–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. 2003. Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torres Cacoullos, R., and J.A. Walker. 2009. The present of the English future: Grammatical variation and collocations in discourse. Language 85 (2): 321–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vadillo, M.A., and H. Matute. 2010. Augmentation in contingency learning under time pressure. British Journal of Psychology 101 (3): 579–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Hamme, L.J., and E.A. Wasserman. 1994. Cue competition in causality judgments: The role of nonpresentation of compound stimulus elements. Learning & Motivation 25 (2): 127–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waldmann, M.R. 2000. Competition among causes but not effects in predictive and diagnostic learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition 26 (1): 53–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldmann, M.R., and K.J. Holyoak. 1992. Predictive and diagnostic learning within causal models: Asymmetries in cue competition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 121 (2): 222–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wasserman, E.A., W.W. Dorner, and S.-F. Kao. 1990. Contributions of specific cell information to judgments to interevent contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16: 509–521.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weir, R.H. 1962. Language in the crib. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J.N. 2003. Inducing abstract linguistic representations: Human and connectionist learning of noun classes. In The lexicon-syntax interface in second language acquisition, ed. R. van Hout, A. Hulk, F. Kuiken, and R.J. Towell, 151–174. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, J., and W.B. Croft. 1998. Corpus-based stemming using co-occurrence of word variants. ACM Transactions on Information Systems 16 (1): 61–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, F., and J.B. Tenenbaum. 2007. Word learning as Bayesian inference. Psychological Review 114 (2): 245–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., and L.B. Smith. 2007. Rapid word learning under uncertainty via cross-situational statistics. Psychological Science 18 (5): 414–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaki, S., A. Rich, and S. Stacy. 2016. The sequence of items in category learning: Modeling and eye-tracking data. Paper presented at the 57th annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Boston, MA, November 17–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zipf, G.K. 1949. Human behavior and the principle of least effort: An introduction to human ecology. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vsevolod Kapatsinski .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Kapatsinski, V. (2018). Learning Morphological Constructions. In: Booij, G. (eds) The Construction of Words. Studies in Morphology, vol 4. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_19

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_19

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-74393-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-74394-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics