Skip to main content

Schema Unification and Morphological Productivity: A Diachronic Perspective

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Construction of Words

Part of the book series: Studies in Morphology ((SUMO,volume 4))

Abstract

Unified schemas which allow for deriving multiply complex word-formation products are a central concept in Construction Morphology (CxM). Based on examples such as un-V-able formations in English, it has been argued in the framework of Construction Morphology that unified schemas (in this case: [un[V-able]A]A) can be conceived of as short cuts in coining new complex words. In this paper, we explore three prospective cases of schema unification and discuss what kind of evidence supports the assumption of unified schemas. The first two case studies are diachronic in nature. Drawing on corpus analyses of data from the Early New High German period (1350–1650) and from the early stages of New High German, we show how the developments of the complex patterns diverge from the developments of their counterparts. To this end, we analyze the frequency and productivity of the (sub-)constructions and assess the semantics of the word-formation products. Firstly, nominalization with the suffix -ung has been shown to undergo a diachronic decrease in morphological productivity. However, unified schemas such as [Be-X-ung]N or [(PREF)-X-ierung]N are shown to be still productive, e.g. Beplankung, Belaberung, Vercomedianisierung (from www.wortwarte.de, a collection of neologisms). In a similar vein, complex derivation of the type [un-V-lich]ADJ ‘un-V-able’ is shown to have remained productive for a longer period of time than its simplex parent schema [V-lich]ADJ. Moreover, many un-V-lich derivatives historically precede their unprefixed counterparts, or lack them altogether (unwiderstehlich ‘irresistible’, but *widerstehlich). Our third case study explores present day German pseudo-participles (bebrillt ‘bespectacled’) using web data. The complex pattern can be shown to diverge stylistically from its parent schemas and also to provide semantically more uniform derivatives. Overall, our results show that the concept of unified schemas can help explain important differences in the development of the individual subpatterns in terms of morphological productivity and in terms of semantic aspects of the word-formation constructions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A reviewer correctly points out that this measure is not without problems. For an in-depth discussion of this issue (including the application of additional measures) see Hartmann (2018).

  2. 2.

    On the divergent development of -lich and -ly in German and English, see Pounder (2001). The early adverbial uses of German -lich are also discussed in detail in Kempf (2016).

  3. 3.

    Note that the last period of the B[onn] corpus and the first period of the M[anchester] corpus coincide. Thus, they were both calculated against the backdrop of the first three periods. Still, the period “1650-1700 M” displays more progressive results, since the corpus contains a higher share of modern genres, i.e. newspapers and scientific texts (see Kempf forthc.).

  4. 4.

    The periods span 50 years each, except for the first two periods, where only coarse-grained dates of attestation were available, e.g. “Middle High German”.

  5. 5.

    The decline of deverbal lich-derivation can be linked, among other things, to the rise of deverbal bar-derivation, cf. Kempf (2016: 189–190) and Flury (1964).

  6. 6.

    Checked in January 2017.

  7. 7.

    “Die Bildungsmöglichkeiten solcher PsPs [=Pseudopartizipien] sind fast unbegrenzt, eine auch nur annähernde Aufstellung solcher Wörter wäre unmöglich”. Van Haeringen (1949: 187) makes the same observation for Dutch pseudo-participles.

  8. 8.

    Using the lemma information available in both the corpus and the n-gram frequency list was not an option as many pseudo-participles are not recognized by taggers and therefore lemmatized as <unknown>. We used the following search pattern: “^be.*(en|t)((er)?e(s|n|m|r)?)”

  9. 9.

    There is one single attestation of the verb bebrillen in the DTA, which dates to 1802. However, it seems unlikely that bebrillt can be regarded as an actual participle, especially given that this one instance is massively outnumbered by the occurrences of the pseudo-participle bebrillt.

  10. 10.

    This is less so with Prf/Prt-X-ung, more so with un-V-lich, and still an open question in the case of pseudo-participles, since the variants with prefixes other than be- still remain to be investigated. Still, the forms are relatively consistent compared, e.g., to suffixes with multiple grapho-phonological variants like ENHG -et/-echt/-ocht/-icht/-igt (cf. Kempf 2016: 74).

  11. 11.

    Record from DECOW; the original website http://forum.scharesoft.de/archive/index.php/t-1085.html is no longer available (2017–01-15).

  12. 12.

    Record from the GerManC corpus, text SERM_P2_NoD_1730_JubelFeste.

  13. 13.

    On paradigmatic relations between constructions, see also Norde & Trousdale (2016).

References

Literature

  • Anderson, J.R., D. Bothell, M.D. Byne, S. Douglass, C. Lebiere, and Y. Quin. 2004. An integrated theory of the mind. Psychological Review 111: 1036–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R.H. 1993. On frequency, transparency, and productivity. In Yearbook of morphology 1992, ed. G. Booij and J. van Marle, 181–208. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2009. Corpus linguistics in morphology: Morphological productivity. In Corpus linguistics (HSK 29.2), ed. A. Lüdeling and M. Kytö, 899–919. Berlin/New York: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barðdal, J. 2008. Productivity: Evidence from case and argument structure in Icelandic, Constructional Approaches to Language 8. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, L. 2001. Morphological productivity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, W.Z. 1992. Pseudopartizipien im deutschen Sprachgebrauch. Ein Nachschlage- und Übungsbuch. Heidelberg: Groos.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biber, D., and E. Finegan. 1997. Diachronic relations among speech-based and written registers in English. In To explain the present. Studies in the changing English language in honour of Matti Rissanen, ed. T. Nevalainen and L. Kahlas-Tarkka, 253–275. Helsinki: Société Néophilologique.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G. 2007. Construction morphology and the lexicon. In Selected Proceedings of the 5th Décembrettes: Morphology in Toulouse, ed. F. Montermini, G. Boyé, and N. Hathout, 34–44. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. Construction morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2015. The nominalization of Dutch particle verbs: Schema unification and second order schemas. Nederlandse Taalkunde 20: 285–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croft, W. 2001. Radical construction grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Demske, U. 2000. Zur Geschichte der ung-Nominalisierung im Deutschen: Ein Wandel morphologischer Produktivität. Beiträge zur Geschichte der deutschen Sprache und Literatur 122: 365–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donalies, E. 2011. Explizite Adjektivderivate. In Grammis 2.0. Das grammatische Informationssystem des Instituts für deutsche Sprache. http://hypermedia.ids-mannheim.de/call/public/sysgram.ansicht?v_id=1231. Last checked 10/02/2017.

  • Durrell, M., A. Ensslin, and P. Bennett. 2007. The GerManC project. Sprache und Datenverarbeitung 31: 71–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, P. 2013. Grundriss der deutschen Grammatik. Bd. 1: Das Wort. 4th ed. Stuttgart: Metzler.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flury, R. 1964. Struktur- und Bedeutungsgeschichte des Adjektiv-Suffixes -bar. Winterthur: Keller. [Dissertation University Zürich].

    Google Scholar 

  • FWB = Frühneuhochdeutsches Wörterbuch. Robert R. Anderson, Ulrich Goebel, Oskar Reichmann, Anja Lobenstein-Reichmann (Eds.). 12 volumes, 1986−. Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ganslmayer, Ch. 2012. Adjektivderivation in der Urkundensprache des 13. Jahrhunderts: Eine historisch-synchrone Untersuchung anhand der ältesten deutschsprachigen Originalurkunden. Berlin/New York: De Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gauger, H.-M. 1994. Geschichte des Lesens. In Schrift und Schriftlichkeit. Ein interdisziplinäres Handbuch internationaler Forschung. 1. Halbband, ed. H. Günther and O. Ludwig, 65–84. Berlin/New York: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giraudo, H., and S. Dal Maso. 2016. The salience of complex words and their parts: Which comes first? Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A.E. 2006. Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosse, S., Grimberg, M, Hölscher, T., and Karweick, J. (eds.). 1989. “Denn das Schreiben gehört nicht zu meiner täglichen Beschäftigung”. Der Alltag kleiner Leute in Bittschriften, Briefen und Berichten aus dem 19. Jahrhundert. Ein Lesebuch. Bonn: Dietz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M.A.K. 2004. The language of science. Collected works of M.A.K. Halliday, vol. 5, ed. J.J. Webster. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, S. 2016. Wortbildungswandel. Eine diachrone Studie zu deutschen Nominalisierungsmustern. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, S. 2018. Derivational morphology in flux. A case study on word-formation change in German. Cognitive Linguistics. Ahead of print, https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2016-0146.

  • Haspelmath, M. 1999. Why is grammaticalization irreversible? Linguistics 37: 1043–1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hay, J.B. 2001. Lexical frequency in morphology: Is everything relative? Linguistics 39: 1041–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2003. Causes and consequences of word structure. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilpert, M. 2014. Construction grammar and its application to English. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2015. From hand-carved to computer-based: Noun-participle compounding and the upward-strengthening hypothesis. Cognitive Linguistics 26: 113–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. forthcoming. Three open questions in diachronic construction grammar. In Grammaticalization meets construction grammar, ed. E. Coussé, P. Andersson, and J. Olofsson. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P. 2015. Emergent grammar. In Handbook of language emergence, ed. B. MacWhinney and W. O’Grady, 314–327. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horlitz, B. 1986. Zur lexikographischen Darstellung der Verbalsubstantive auf -ung. In Studien zur neuhochdeutschen Lexikographie VI, Teilbd. 1 (GL 87), ed. H.E. Wiegand, 479–490. Hildesheim/Zürich/New York: Olms.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hüning, M., and B. Schlücker. 2010. Konvergenz und Divergenz in der Wortbildung. Komposition im Niederländischen und Deutschen. In Kontrastive germanistische Linguistik, ed. A. Dammel, S. Kürschner, and D. Nübling, 783–825. Hildesheim: Olms.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, R. 1994. Sprachwandel: Von der unsichtbaren Hand in der Sprache. Tübingen/Basel: Francke.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kempf, L. 2016. Adjektivsuffixe in Konkurrenz. Wortbildungswandel vom Frühneuhochdeutschen zum Neuhochdeutschen. Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, Th., H.-J. Solms, and K.-P. Wegera, eds. 2009. Mittelhochdeutsche Grammatik: Wortbildung. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoop, U. 1994. Entwicklung von Literalität und Alphabetisierung in Deutschland. In Schrift und Schriftlichkeit. Ein interdisziplinäres Handbuch internationaler Forschung. 1. Halbband, ed. H. Günther and O. Ludwig, 859–872. Berlin/New York: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, P. 2004. Sprachwandel, Mündlichkeit und Schriftlichkeit. Zeitschrift für romanische Philologie 120: 605–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, P., and W. Oesterreicher. 1996. Sprachwandel und expressive Mündlichkeit. Zeitschrift für Literaturwissenschaft und Linguistik 102: 64–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kopf, K. 2016. Mainzer (Früh-)Neuhochdeutschkorpus (1500–1710). Mainz: University of Mainz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kühnhold, I., O. Putzer, and H. Wellmann. 1978. Deutsche Wortbildung. Typen und Tendenzen in der Gegenwartssprache. Eine Bestandsaufnahme des Instituts für Deutsche Sprache, Forschungsstelle Innsbruck. Dritter Hauptteil: Das Adjektiv. Düsseldorf: Schwann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kupietz, M., C. Belica, H. Keibel, and A. Witt. 2010. The German Reference Corpus DeReKo: A primordial sample for linguistic research. In Proceedings of the 7th Conference on International Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2010)., ed. N. Calzolari et al., 1848–1854. Valletta: European Language Resources Association (ELRA). http://www.lrec-conf.org/proceedings/lrec2010/pdf/414_Paper.pdf.

  • Langacker, R.W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motsch, W. 2004. Deutsche Wortbildung in Grundzügen. 2nd ed. Berlin/New York: de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Norde, M., and G. Trousdale. 2016. Exaptation from the perspective of construction morphology. In Exaptation and language change, ed. M. Norde and F. Van de Velde, 163–195. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, H. 1897. Ueber die Aufgaben der Wortbildungslehre. Sitzungsberichte der philosophisch-philologischen und der historischen Classe der königlich bayerischen Akademie der Wissenschaften 1896, Heft IV, 692–713. München: Staub.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfeifer, W. 1993. Etymologisches Wörterbuch des Deutschen. Online version via https://www.dwds.de/.

  • Pimenova, N.B. 2002. Semantische Restriktionen für die unga-Ableitung im Althochdeutschen. Germanistisches Jahrbuch der GUS “Das Wort”, 93–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plag, I. 1999. Morphological productivity: Structural constraints in English derivation, Topics in English Linguistics 28. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2005. Word-formation in English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plag, I., Ch. Dalton-Puffer, and R.H. Baayen. 1999. Morphological productivity across speech and writing. English Language and Linguistics 3: 209–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pleyer, M. 2017. Protolanguage and mechanisms of meaning construal in interaction. Language Sciences 63: 69–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pounder, A.V. 2001. Adverb-marking in German and English: System and standardization. Diachronica 18: 301–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riecke, J. 1999. Pseudopartizipien im Althochdeutschen. Ein Beitrag zur Geschichte eines Wortbildungstyps. Sprachwissenschaft 24: 157–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D.E. 1980. Schemata: The building blocks of cognition. In Theoretical issues in reading comprehension: Perspectives from cognitive psychology, linguistics, artificial intelligence, and education, ed. R.J. Spiro, B.C. Bruce, and W.F. Brewer, 33–58. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schäfer, R., and F. Bildhauer. 2012. Building large corpora from the web using a new efficient tool chain. In Proceedings of LREC 2012, ed. C. Calzolari, K. Choukri, T. Declerck, M. Uğur Doğan, B. Maegaard, J. Mariani, A. Moreno, J. Odijk, and S. Piperidis, 486–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, C. 2005. Wortbildungswandel und Produktivität. Eine empirische Studie zur nominalen -er-Derivation im Deutschen. Tübingen: Niemeyer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid, H.-J. 2007. Entrenchment, salience, and basic levels. In The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics, ed. D. Geeraerts and H. Cuyckens, 118–138. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2014. A blueprint of the entrenchment-and-conventionalization model. In Yearbook of the German Cognitive Linguistics Association, ed. M. Hilpert and S. Flach, vol. 2, 3–25. Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2017. A framework for understanding entrenchment and its psychological foundations. In Entrenchment and the psychology of language learning. How we reorganize and adapt linguistic knowledge, ed. H.-J. Schmid, 9–39. Berlin/Boston: de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schmid, H.-J., and F. Günther. 2016. Toward a unified socio-cognitive framework for salience in language. Frontiers in Psychology 7. http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01110.

  • Schröder, M. 1988. Entwicklungstendenzen in der Wortbildung. In Entwicklungstendenzen in der deutschen Gegenwartssprache, ed. K.-E. Sommerfeldt, 174–192. Leipzig: Bibliographisches Institut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szczepaniak, R. 2015. Syntaktische Einheitenbildung – typologisch und diachron betrachtet. In Handbuch Satz, Äußerung, Schema, ed. Ch. Dürscheid and J.G. Schneider, 104–124. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J.R. 2002. Cognitive grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, B. 2002. Adjektivderivation im Nürnberger Frühneuhochdeutsch um 1500: Eine historisch-synchrone Analyse anhand von Texten Albrecht Dürers, Veit Dietrichs und Heinrich Deichslers. Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Traugott, E.C., and G. Trousdale. 2013. Constructionalization and constructional changes, Oxford studies in diachronic and historical linguistics 6. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Van de Velde, F. 2014. Degeneracy: The maintenance of constructional networks. In Extending the scope of construction grammar, ed. R. Boogaart, T. Colleman, and G. Rutten, 141–180. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Haeringen, C.B. 1949. Participia praeverbalia. De Nieuwe Taalgids 42: 187–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber, H. 1971. Das erweiterte Adjektiv- und Partizipialattribut im Deutschen. München: Hueber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, R. 1960. Nominal and verbal style. In Style in language, ed. T.A. Sebeok, 213–220. Cambridge, MA/New York: Technology Press & Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkler, G. 1995. Die Wortbildung mit -lich im Alt-, Mittel- und Frühneuhochdeutschen. Heidelberg: Winter.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Geert Booij and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on a previous version of this paper. Any remaining errors and shortcomings are of course ours.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Luise Kempf .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendix

Appendix

Table 3 Sample of ung-derivatives arranged by first attestationa
Table 4 Deverbal passive lich-derivatives arranged by first attestationa

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Kempf, L., Hartmann, S. (2018). Schema Unification and Morphological Productivity: A Diachronic Perspective. In: Booij, G. (eds) The Construction of Words. Studies in Morphology, vol 4. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_16

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-74394-3_16

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-74393-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-74394-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics