Skip to main content

The Role of Verb Semantic Representation in Sentence Processing in Children with SLI

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-speaking Children

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 14))

  • 844 Accesses

Abstract

Background and rationale. Different lexical processing theories assume that the recognition of a word leads to the automatic activation of a range of syntactic and semantic knowledge that are relevant to the interpretation of language. In this respect, the verb is a key word in language processing and in verbal representation. The activation of a verb includes various types of information (subcategorization frame, argument structure, information on thematic roles and lexical-conceptual information), which are activated in real time when a verb is retrieved in processes of comprehension and language production. In specific terms, this information plays an important role in the anticipation of information in processes of understanding and in the degree of correction in production tasks.

This chapter reviews various studies exploring the role of verb semantics and argument structure in language comprehension and production tasks. We studied if there are differences between receptive and expressive in children with SLI using different methodologies. Moreover, we analyzed if verb semantics is impaired in children with SLI and if we can found that the problems are due to impoverished verb semantic representations or limitations in language processing.

Results and Discussion. The amount of information represented in verbs, and their use in language comprehension and production tasks was analyzed in various studies. Children with SLI showed more difficulties in language production than in language comprehension. In receptive language, although the results vary depending on the task, it can be said that children with SLI have incomplete semantic representations for verbs which affect processing times, correct naming and the omission of arguments. Furthermore, the results show that lexical-semantic representations by children with SLI are similar to those among younger children, and as such it would be more accurate to describe it in terms of a pattern of delay rather than a disorder.

Results show that children with SLI present longer processing times, lower levels of correction in naming and larger percentages of omission of verbal arguments compared to the control children. These results suggest that children with SLI have incomplete semantic representations of verbs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., & Guàrdia, J. (2012a). Auditory word recognition of nouns and verbs in children with specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Communication Disorders, 45(1), 20–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., Buil, L., & MacWhinney, B. (2012b). Effect of verb argument structure on picture naming in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Communication Disorders, 47(6), 637–653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., & Trueswell, J. (2013). Anticipatory sentence processing in children with SLI: Evidence from eye movements during listening. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 34, 5–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., & Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J. (2016). Do children with SLI use verbs to predict arguments and adjuncts: Evidence from eye movements during listening. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1917. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. (1997). Uncommon understanding. London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children's memory: An interpretive review. Developmental Review, 7, 93–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, G., & Tanenhaus, M. (1988). Thematic roles and language comprehension. In W. Wilkins (Ed.), Thematic relations. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., & Jones, M. (1997). Verb use in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 1298–1313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, J. (1999). Specific language impairment in Dutch: Inflectional morphology and argument structure. Enxchede: Print Partners Ipskamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dockrell, J. E., Messer, D., George, R., & Ralli, A. (2003). Beyond naming patterns in children with WFDs–definitions for nouns and verbs. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16, 191–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, P. (1991). Evidence from syntax for language impairment. In Miller (Ed.), Research on child language disorders (pp. 167–187). Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, P., & Garman, M. (1988). Normal language development and language impairment: Syntax and beyond. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 2, 97–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grela, B. G. (2003). The omission of subject arguments in children with Specific Language Impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17(2), 153–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grela, B. G., & Leonard, L. B. (1997). The use of subject arguments by children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 11, 443–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J., & Kamhi, A. (1984). Syntactic and semantic aspects of the utterances of language-impaired children: The same can be less. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 30, 65–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R., & Leonard, L. B. (1986). Word-finding abilities in language-impaired children, ASHA Monographs, No. 25. Rockville: American Speech-Language Hearing Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, G., & Fletcher, P. (1993). Grammatical problems in school-age children with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 7, 339–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahey, M., & Edwards, J. (1996). Why do children with specific language impairment name pictures more slowly than their peers? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39, 1081–1098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., & Nye, C. (2000). Prevalence and neutral story of primary speech and language delay: Findings from a systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 35, 165–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, L. (1976). Normal and atypical semantic development. Unpublished manuscript, Northwestern University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. (1998). Specific language impairment. London: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. (2014). Specific language impairment. Second edition. London: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B., Nippold, M. A., Kail, R., & Hale, C. A. (1983). Picture naming in language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 609–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L., Eyer, J., Bedore, L., & Grela, B. (1997). Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 741–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2007). Morphological processes in language comprehension. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 175–193). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, K. K., & Appel, A. (2002). On the relation between mental representation and naming in a child with specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 16, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, K. K., Newman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., & Capone, N. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 998–1014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C., Kail, R., Leonard, L., & Tomblin, B. (2001). Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(2), 416–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J. (2000). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2), 293–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moss, H. E., Tyler, L. K., & Taylor, K. I. (2007). Conceptual structure. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 217–234). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pizzioli, F., & Schelstraete, M. A. (2008). The argument-structure complexity effect in children with specific language impairment: Evidence from the use of grammatical morphemes in French. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 706–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M., & Bode, J. (1993). GAPS in the verb lexicons of children with specific language impairment. First Language, 13, 113–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of a Specific Language Impairment in English speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39, 1239–1257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M., & Wexler, K. (1997). Highly specified and long-term grammatical deficits appear in children with language impairments. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitives. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(850–863), 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  • Royle, P., Jarema, G., & Kehayia, E. (2002). Frequency effects on visual word access in developmental language impairment. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 15, 11–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sabish, B., Hahne, A., Glass, E., von Suchodoletz, W., & Friederici, A. (2006). Lexical-semantic processes in children with specific language impairment. Neuroreport, 17, 1511–1514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanz-Torrent, M., Andreu, L., Badia, I., & Sidera, F. (2011). Argument omissions in preschool Catalan and Spanish speaking children with SLI. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 34(1), 49–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (2007). Connectionist models of reading. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 235–250). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. P. (2003). Argument structure: Representation and processing. In R. Kent (Ed.), The encyclopedia of communication sciences and disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. P., & Nagel, H. N. (1995). Lexical properties, prosody, and syntax: Implications for normal and disordered language. Brain and Language, 50, 240–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheng, L., & McGregor, K. (2010). Object and action naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 1704–1719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thordardottir, E., & Weismer, S. E. (2002). Verb argument structure weakness in specific language impairment in relation to age and utterance length. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 16(4), 233–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, A. L., & Van Balkom, J. (2004). Classification of developmental language disorders. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weismer, S. E., Evans, J., & Hesketh, L. (1999). An examination of verbal working memory capacity in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 42, 1249–1260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wren, C. (1982). Identifying patterns of processing disorder in six-year-old children with syntax problems. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 17(3), 83–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Llorenç Andreu .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M. (2017). The Role of Verb Semantic Representation in Sentence Processing in Children with SLI. In: Auza Benavides, A., Schwartz, R. (eds) Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-speaking Children. Literacy Studies, vol 14. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-53646-0_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-53646-0_11

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-53645-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-53646-0

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics