Skip to main content

Temporal Coherence in Discourse: Theory and Application for Machine Translation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Formal Models in the Study of Language

Abstract

Temporal coherence in discourse is provided through several temporal cohesive ties, such as tense, aspect and discourse connectives. In the relevance theoretic framework and more specifically in the Geneva school of pragmatics, these cohesive ties are considered as encoding procedural information important for guiding the hearer towards the intended interpretation of the discourse. Jacques Moeschler and his team studied temporal cohesive ties and proposed original theoretical models that have been validated with human and automatic annotation experiments, as well as in language acquisition studies (Zufferey and Popescu-Belis, this volume). In this paper, I show that Jacques Moeschler’s model for inferring temporal discourse relations and his description of tenses expressing past time in French is cross-linguistically valid and can be modelled for improving the results of statistical machine translation systems.

The ideas presented in this paper are based on a series of articles published with Bruno Cartoni, Thomas Meyer, Andrei Popescu-Belis, Michele Costagliola and Jacques Moeschler, with whom I collaborated on two research projects. I am very grateful to Jacques Moeschler for his guidance and resourceful discussions since the beginning of my research. A theoretical model of temporal reference in tensed languages based on empirical work (corpus-based and experiments with native speakers) is proposed in my Ph.D. dissertation (Grisot 2015), in which I make the proposal that temporal coherence in discourse is triggered by the hearer’s need to acquire temporal coherence at the cognitive level. In order to attain this purpose, he treats information coming from several sources (tense, grammatical aspect, lexical aspect, temporal adverbials and temporal connectives) and their rich interrelations in a coherent manner.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 99.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The COMTIS Project (Improving the Coherence of Machine Translation Output by Modeling Intersentential Relations; project n° CRSI22_127510, March 2010–July 2013) and the MODERN Project (Modeling discourse entities and relations for coherent machine translation; project n° CRSII2_147653, August 2013–August 2016) belong to the Sinergia interdisciplinary program funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation.

  2. 2.

    At the beginning of the eighties, the label “Geneva School” was given to a series of publications on discourse and conversation that applied basic principles of syntactic analysis to the domain of discourse (Roulet et al. 1985; Moeschler 1985). In the beginning of the nineties, two different directions could be identified in the Geneva School: (i) a general discourse-oriented framework of language based on the modular hypothesis (Roulet 1997) and (ii) a radical pragmatic perspective on discourse sequencing and discourse interpretation (Moeschler 1993, 1996) (see detailed presentation in Moeschler 2001).

  3. 3.

    For example Gazdar (1979), Horn (1984; 1992; 2004; 2007), Levinson (1983; 2000).

  4. 4.

    For example Sperber and Wilson (1986/1995), Blakemore (1987, 2002), Carston (2002).

  5. 5.

    I assume that this specialization conducted Kamp and Rohrer (1983) to argue that the PS encodes a forward temporal inference. I argue that the PS encodes a procedure regarding directional temporal inference. In other words, it is an instruction for the hearer to ascertain the contextual value of the directional temporal inference.

  6. 6.

    For a development of the conceptualist view of tense based on experimental findings using the cognitive features of conceptual and procedural information proposed by Wilson and Sperber (1993), see Grisot (2015).

  7. 7.

    The narrativity feature is a coarse grained semantic and pragmatic feature proposed in a particular framework, that of Natural Language Processing and Machine Translation. I admit that from a theoretical point of view, only finer coarse features can explain all possible (both very frequent and less frequent) usages of a verb tense.

  8. 8.

    Verb tense has frequently been associated with narrative contexts in various frameworks, such as in DRT and SDRT. Smith (2003) discussed discourse modes based in textual structure and aspect. If these theories focused on linguistic information and made use of non-monotonic inferences, for us narrativity is procedural information representing a cognitive (as opposed to logic in (S)DRT) discourse relation (Hobbs 1979; Mann and Thompson 1988; Sanders et al. 1992). Cognitive discourse relations are expressed lexically through verb tense and connectives (that are language-specific) and can occur in any type of stylistic register.

  9. 9.

    At this point of the research, I consider lexical aspect as one class and do not distinguish between Vendler (1957). Moeschler (2000) discusses Dowty’s principle of interpretation of temporal discourses (1986) based on lexical aspect. Moeschler (2000) argues that this approach to temporal relations adopts a radical position and does not explain a certain number of exceptions.

  10. 10.

    The translation of the SP through a present tense form can be explained by the contextual values taken by temporal coordinates S, R and E in order to lead to the speaker’s intended interpretation. Specifically, the translation with present time signals that the eventuality is viewed from the moment of speech (R = S) (see Grisot and Moeschler, submitted for publication, where we argue based on experimental results that temporal coordinates are conceptual information).

  11. 11.

    Samardzic (2013) uses this novel methodology for investigating the translation equivalents of a range of English light verb constructions into several languages. Slavic languages encode verb aspect lexically, unlike other European languages. She applies the aspectual representation obtained in the English-Serbian cross-linguistic setting to classify English verbs into event duration classes.

  12. 12.

    These results include only cases where inter-annotator agreement is high. Four participants judged each sentence and the result is based on the majority of answers.

  13. 13.

    BLEU (Bilingual Evaluation Understudy) is an evaluation measure for machine-translated texts. It calculates the degree of resemblance to a human-translated text and it is a number between 0 and 1, where values closer to 1 represent more similar texts.

  14. 14.

    Some of these suggestions are investigated in (Grisot 2015).

References

  • Asher N (1993) Reference to abstract objects in discourse. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Asher N, Lascarides A (2003) Logics of conversation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahern A, Leonetti M (2004) The Spanish subjunctive: procedural semantics and pragmatic inference. In: Pragmatics and beyond, vol 123, pp 35–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Amenós-Pons J (2010) Telling a story in French and in Spanish: past tenses and temporal relations revisited. Pragmatic perspectives on language and linguistics I: speech actions in theory and applied studies, pp 395–424

    Google Scholar 

  • Amenos-Pons J (2011) Cross-linguistic variation in procedural expressions: semantics and pragmatics. In: Escandell-Vidal V, Leonetti M, Ahern A (eds) Procedural meaning: problems and perspectives. Emerald Group Publishing Limited, Bingley, pp 235–266

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore D (1987) Semantic constraints on relevance. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore D (2002) Relevance and linguistic meaning: the semantics and pragmatics of discourse markers. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Carston R (1988) Implicature, explicature, and truth-theoretic semantics. In: Kempson R (ed) Mental representations: the interface between language and reality. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 155–181

    Google Scholar 

  • Carston R (1993) Conjunction, explanation and relevance. Lingua 90(1):27–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Carston R (2002) Thoughts and utterances: the pragmatics of explicit communication. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen LJ (1971) The logical particles of natural languages. In: Bar-Hillel Y (ed) Pragmatics of natural language. Reidel, Dordrecht, pp 50–68

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure L (1997) Passé simple et encapsulation d’événements. Cahiers de linguistique française 19:323–344

    Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure L (2000) Quand le temps ne progresse pas avec le passé simple. Cahiers Chronos 6:37–48

    Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure L (2003) Temps et pertinence: éléments de pragmatique cognitive du temps. De Boeck, Bruxelles

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gazdar G (1979) Pragmatics: Implicature, presupposition, and logical form. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice HP (1975) Logic and conversation. In: Cole P, Morgan J (eds) Speech acts, pp 41–58

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice HP (1967) Presupposition and conversational implicature. In: Cole P (ed) Radical pragmatics. Academic Press, New York, pp 183–198

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice HP (1967) Logic and conversation: The william james lectures. Harvard University, MS

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice HP (1989) Study in the way of words. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Grimes JE (1975) The thread of discourse. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Grisot C (2015) Temporal reference: empirical and theoretical perspectives. Dissertation, University of Geneva

    Google Scholar 

  • Grisot C, Cartoni B (2012) Une description bilingue des temps verbaux: étude contrastive en corpus. Cahiers de linguistique française 30:101–117

    Google Scholar 

  • Grisot C, Costagliola M (2014) What do translation corpora tell us about the semantics and pragmatics of tense? The case of the English preterit. Chronos 11, 16–18 Jun 2014. University of Pise, Italy

    Google Scholar 

  • Grisot C, Moeschler J (2014) How do empirical methods interact with theoretical pragmatics? The conceptual and procedural contents of the English Simple Past and its translation into French. In: Romero-Trillo J (ed) Yearbook of corpus linguistics and pragmatics 2014: new empirical and theoretical paradigms. Springer International Publishing, Switzerland, pp 7–33

    Google Scholar 

  • Grisot C, Meyer T (2014) Cross-linguistic manual and automatic annotation for a pragmatic feature of verb tense. In: Proceedings of LREC, 28–30 May 2014, Reykjavik, Island

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosz BJ, Sidner CL (1986) Attention, intentions, and the structure of discourse. Comput Linguist 12(3):175–204

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday M, Hasan R (1976) Cohesion in English. Longman, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobbs J (1979) Coherence and coreference. Cogn Sci 3:67–90

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobbs JR (1985) On the coherence and structure of discourse. Center for the study of language and information. Stanford University, pp 85–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn L (1984) Toward a new taxonomy for pragmatic inference: Q-based and R-based implicature. In: Schiffrin D (ed) Meaning, form, and use in context. Georgetown University Press, Washington DC, pp 11–42

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn L (1992) The said and the unsaid. Ohio State University Working Papers in Linguistics. 40:163–192

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn L (2004) Implicature. In: Horn L, Ward G (eds) The handbook of pragmatics. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 3–28

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn LR (2006) The border wars: a neo-gricean perspective. In: von Heusinger K, Turner K (eds) Where semantics meets pragmatics: the Michigan papers. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 21–48

    Google Scholar 

  • Horn LR (2007) Toward a fregean pragmatics: Voraussetzung, nebengedanke, andeutung. In: Kecskes I, Horn L (eds) Explorations in pragmatics: linguistic, cognitive and intercultural aspects. Mouton de Gruyter, Berlin, pp 39–69

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamp H, Reyle U (1993) From discourse to logic: introduction to modeltheoretic semantics of natural language, formal logic and discourse representation. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamp H, Rohrer C (1983) Tense in texts. In: Bauerle R, Schwarze C, von Stechow A (eds) Meaning, use and interpretation of language. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin, pp 250–269

    Google Scholar 

  • Kehler A (2004) Discourse coherence. In: Horn L, Ward G (eds) The handbook of pragmatics. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 241–265

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov W, Waletzky J (1967) Narrative analysis: oral versions of personal experience. In: Helm J (ed) Essays on the verbal and visual arts: proceedings of the 1966 annual spring meeting of the American Ethnological Society. University of Washington Press, Seattle, pp 12–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Lascarides A, Asher N (1993) Temporal interpretation, discourse relations and common-sense entailment. Linguist Philos 16:437–493

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonetti M, Escandell-Vidal V (2003) On the quotative readings of Spanish imperfect. Cuaderons Linguist 10:135–154

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson SC (1983) Pragmatics. Cambridge textbooks in linguistics, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson SC (2000) Presumptive meanings: the theory of generalized conversational implicature. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Longacre R (1983) Vertical threads of cohesion in discourse. In: Neubauer F (ed) Coherence in natural-language texts. Helmut Buske Verlag, Hambourg, pp 99–113

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann WC, Thompson SA (1986) Relational propositions in discourse. Discourse Process 9(1):57–90

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann WC, Thompson SA (1988) Rhetorical structure theory: toward a functional theory of text organization. Text 8(3):243–281

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer T, Grisot C, Popescu-Belis A (2013) Detecting narrativity to improve English/French translation of simple past verbs. In: Proceedings of DiscoMT 2013 (ACL workshop on discourse in machine translation), Sofia, Bulgaria, pp 33–42

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler J (1985) Argumentation et conversation: éléments pour une analyse pragmatique du discours. Hatier, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler J (1993) Aspects pragmatiques de la référence temporelle: indétermination, ordre temporel et inference. Langages 112:39–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler J (1994) Connaissances d’arrière-plan et accessibilité contextuelle dans l’interaction verbale. In: Trognon A et al (eds) La construction interactive du quotidien. Presses Universitaires de Nancy, Nancy, pp 69–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler J (1996) Théorie pragmatique et pragmatique conversationnelle. Armand Colin, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler J (2000) L’ordre temporel est-il naturel? Narration, causalité et temps verbaux. In: Moeschler J, Béguelin M-J (eds) Référence temporelle et nominale. Peter Lang, Berne, pp 71–105

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler J (2001) The Geneva school. In: Brinker K, Antos G, Heinnemann W, Sager SF (eds) Linguistics of text and conversation. An international handbook of contemporary research. Walter de Gruyter, Berlin, pp 952–957

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler J (2002) Economy and pragmatic optimality: the case of directional inferences. Gener Grammar Geneva 3:1–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler J (2005) Connecteurs pragmatiques, inférences directionnelles et représentations mentales. In: Molendijk A, Vet C (eds) Temporalité et attitude: structuration du discours et expression de la modalité. Cahiers Chronos 12, pp 35–50

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler J, Grisot C, Cartoni B (2012) Jusqu’où les temps verbaux sont-ils procéduraux? Nouveaux Cahiers de linguistique française 30:119–139

    Google Scholar 

  • Moeschler JH, Jayez, J-M, Luscher, de Saussure LL, Sthioul B, Kozłowska M (1998) Le Temps des événements: Pragmatique de la référence temporelle. Kimé, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolle S (1997) Conceptual and procedural encoding: criteria for the identification of linguistically encoded procedural information. In: Groefsema M (ed) Proceedings of University of Hertfordshire relevance theory workshop. Peter Thomas and Associates, Chelmsford, pp 45–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolle S (1998) A relevance theory perspective on grammaticalization. Cogn Linguist 9:1–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reboul A, Moeschler J (1998) Pragmatique du discours. De l’interprétation de l’énoncé à l’interprétation du discours. Armand Colin, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Reichenbach H (1947) Symbolic logic. University of California, Berkeley

    Google Scholar 

  • Roulet E (1997) A modular approach to discourse structures. Pragmatics 7:125–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roulet E, Auchlin A, Moeschler J, Rubattel C, Schelling M (1985) L’articulation du discours en français contemporain. Peter Lang, Berne

    Google Scholar 

  • Samaradzic T (2013) Dynamics, causation, duration in the predicate-argument structure of verbs: a computational approach based on parallel corpora. Dissertation, University of Geneva

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders T (1997) Semantic and pragmatic sources of coherence: on the categorization of coherence relations in context. Discourse Process 24:119–147

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders T (2005) Coherence, causality and cognitive complexity in discourse. In: Proceedings/Actes SEM-05, first international symposium on the exploration and modelling of meaning, pp 105–114

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders T, Spooren W, Noordman L (1992) Toward a taxonomy of coherence relations. Discourse Process 15:1–35

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanders T, Noordman L (2000) The role of coherence relations and their linguistic markers in text processing. Discourse Process 29:37–60

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure L (2011) On some methodological issues in the conceptual/procedural distinction. In: Escandell-Vidal V, Leonetti M, Ahern A (eds) Procedural meaning: problems and perspectives. Emerald Group Publishing Limited, Bingley, pp 55–79

    Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure L, Sthioul B (1999) L’imparfait narratif: point de vue (et images du monde). Cahiers de praxématique 32:167–188

    Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure L, Sthioul B (2005) Imparfait et enrichissement pragmatique. Cahiers Chronos 14:103–120

    Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure L, Morency P (2012) A cognitive-pragmatic view of the French epistemic future. J French Lang Stud 22:207–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith N (1990) Observations on the pragmatics of tense. UCL working papers in linguistics, pp 113–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith CS (2003) Modes of discourse: the local structure of texts. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D, Wilson D (1986/1995) Relevance: communication and cognition. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Tahara I (2000) Le passé simple et la subjectivité. Cahiers de linguistique française 22:189–218

    Google Scholar 

  • Vendler Z (1957) Linguistics in philosophy. Cornell University Press, Ithaca

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson D, Sperber D (1993) Linguistic form and relevance. Lingua 90:1–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson D, Sperber D (1998) Pragmatics and time. In: Carston R, Uchida S (eds) Relevance theory: applications and implications. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 1–22

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cristina Grisot .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Grisot, C. (2017). Temporal Coherence in Discourse: Theory and Application for Machine Translation. In: Blochowiak, J., Grisot, C., Durrleman, S., Laenzlinger, C. (eds) Formal Models in the Study of Language. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-48832-5_19

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-48832-5_19

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-48831-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-48832-5

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics