Skip to main content

Cognitive Load Theory, Evolutionary Educational Psychology, and Instructional Design

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Evolutionary Perspectives on Child Development and Education

Part of the book series: Evolutionary Psychology ((EVOLPSYCH))

Abstract

Evolutionary educational psychology provides a framework for more fully understanding what we learn, how we learn, and informs us of the instructional procedures that should work in modern schools. Knowledge can be categorized into biologically primary knowledge that can be acquired automatically through natural activities such as play without explicit tuition and biologically secondary knowledge that needs to be taught explicitly. During secondary learning, human cognition constitutes a natural information processing system that has evolved to mimic the architecture of biological evolution. Cognitive load theory uses this architecture to generate a large range of instructional effects concerned with procedures for reducing extraneous working memory load in order to facilitate the acquisition of secondary knowledge in long-term memory. This chapter reviews the evolutionary and cognitive bases of the theory and summarizes some of the instructional effects generated.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). Oxford, England: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewoods Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1961). The art of discovery. Harvard Educational Review, 31, 21–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cahan, S., & Cohen, N. (1989). Age versus schooling effects on intelligence development. Child Development, 60, 1239–1249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 55–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87–114.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (2003). The descent of man. London, England: Gibson Square (Original work published 1871).

    Google Scholar 

  • De Groot, A. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton (Original work published 1946).

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, D. E., & Schwartz, B. J. (1979). Chunking in recall of symbolic drawings. Memory & Cognition, 7, 149–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, J. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171–191. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50, 24–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. (2005). The origin of mind: Evolution of brain, cognition, and general intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. (2012). Evolutionary educational psychology. In K. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook (Vol. 1, pp. 597–621). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2005). Evolution in four dimensions: genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeffries, R., Turner, A., Polson, P., & Atwood, M. (1981). Processes involved in designing software. In J. R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 255–283). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner, P., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41, 75–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., & Nigam, M. (2004). The equivalence of learning paths in early science instruction: Effects of direct instruction and discovery learning. Psychological Science, 15, 661–667.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? The case for guided methods of instruction. American Psychologist, 59, 14–19.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, L., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short-term retention of individual verbal items. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 193–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. (1979). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford, England: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Portin, P. (2002). Historical development of the concept of the gene. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, 27, 257–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, S., Bates, T., & Deary, I. (2015). Is education associated with improvements in general cognitive ability or in specific skills? Developmental Psychology, 51(5), 573–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children’s thinking. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stotz, K., & Griffiths, P. (2004). Genes: Philosophical analyses put to the test. History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences, 26, 5–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2009). Cognitive bases of human creativity. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 11–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 123–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York, NY: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 59–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Sweller, S. (2006). Natural information processing systems. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 434–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, A., & Raihani, N. (2008). The evolution of teaching. Animal Behaviour, 75, 1823–1836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tricot, A., & Sweller, J. (2014). Domain-specific knowledge and why teaching generic skills does not work. Educational Psychology Review, 26, 265–283. doi:10.1007/s10648-013-9243-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Sweller .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sweller, J. (2016). Cognitive Load Theory, Evolutionary Educational Psychology, and Instructional Design. In: Geary, D., Berch, D. (eds) Evolutionary Perspectives on Child Development and Education. Evolutionary Psychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-29986-0_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics