Skip to main content

Signaling in Style: On Cooperation, Identity and the Origins of Visual Art

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Understanding Cultural Traits

Abstract

This paper argues that visual art coevolved with typically human ways of social organization and cooperation strategies. My argument, in brief, is that Late Pleistocene human groups became organised in band societies that established networks of indirect reciprocal cooperation, which favoured cultural strategies of individual recognition such as social markers, e.g. styles of personal ornamentation. These early forms of visual art, by conveying information about social identity, became important in recalling and assessing individual interactions in cooperative networks. I also argue that as a cultural strategy, visual art could have been adaptive by reducing risk of aggression and increasing resource acquisition through exchange. As other evolved cultural traits, like tool-making and cooking, visual art too could have had an important impact on shaping modern human cognition and behaviour.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Connection: Chaps. 20 and 21 address in similar evolutionary ‘whys and hows’ of human traits that determine the capacity for cultural variation: aesthetic preferences and literature.

  2. 2.

    Connection: Sect. 10.3 offers a philosophical analysis of style – for example artistic or writing style – as not planned, repeated out of control, and recognized in hindsight and on a large observation scale.

  3. 3.

    Connection: This Chapter often recalls the crucial role played by archaeology. Please, refer to Chap. 15 for a theoretical introduction to the role of archeology in identifying cultures and cultural traits.

  4. 4.

    Connection: The hypothesis of visual art as a signal to obtain cooperation in complex societies is a good example of applying Evolutionary Game Theory (Sect. 12.3) and its models of evolution of cooperative behaviors.

  5. 5.

    For a review of the different uses of ‘style’ in archaeology, see: M. Conkey (1990, 2006).

  6. 6.

    Connection: A discussion of values as cultural traits is found in Sect. 14.3 and Chap. 12.

References

  • Aiello, L. C., & Dunbar, R. I. (1993). Neocortex size, group size, and the evolution of language. Current Anthropology, 34(2), 184–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aiken, N. E. (1998). The biological origins of art. Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambrose, S. H. (2010). Coevolution of composite-tool technology, constructive memory, and language: Implications for the evolution of modern human behavior. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S135–S147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aureli, F., Schaffner, C. M., Boesch, C., Bearder, S. K., Call, J., Chapman, C. A., Connor, R., Di Fiore, A., Dunbar, R. I., & Henzi, S. P. (2008). Fission fusion dynamics. Current Anthropology, 49(4), 627–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnard, A. (2009). Social origins: Sharing, exchange, kinship. In R. Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The cradle of language (pp. 219–235). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barham, L. S. (1998). Possible early pigment use in South Central Africa. Current Anthropology, 39(5), 703–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barham, L., & Mitchell, P. (2008). The first Africans: African archaeology from the earliest tool makers to most recent foragers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barton, C. M., Clark, G. A., & Allison, E. C. (1994). Art as information: Explaining upper Palaeolithic art in western Europe. World Archaeology, 26(2), 185–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2011). A cooperative species: Human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Breuil, H., & Windels, F. (1952). Quatre cents siècles d’art pariétal: les cavernes ornées de l’âge du renne. Montignac: Centre d’études et de documentation préhistoriques.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckley, C., & Steele, J. (2002). Evolutionary ecology of spoken language: Co-evolutionary hypotheses are testable. World Archaeology, 34(1), 26–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunge, M. (2003). Emergence and convergence: Qualitative novelty and the unity of knowledge. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, P. G. (1991). Symbols and paleolithic artifacts: Style, standardization, and the imposition of arbitrary form. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 10(3), 193–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clottes, J., & Arnold, M. (2003). Chauvet cave: The art of earliest times. Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coe, K. (2003). The ancestress hypothesis: Visual art as adaptation. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conard, N. J. (2003). Palaeolithic ivory sculptures from southwestern Germany and the origins of figurative art. Nature, 426(6968), 830–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conard, N. J. (2009). A female figurine from the basal Aurignacian of Hohle Fels Cave in southwestern Germany. Nature, 459(7244), 248–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conard, N. J., & Bolus, M. (2003). Radiocarbon dating the appearance of modern humans and timing of cultural innovations in Europe: New results and new challenges. Journal of Human Evolution, 44(3), 331–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conkey, M. (1990). Experimenting with style in archaeology. In M. Conkey & C. Hastorf (Eds.), The uses of style in archaeology (pp. 5–17). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conkey, M. W. (2006). Style, design and function. In C. Tilley et al. (Eds.), Handbook of material culture (pp. 355–372). London: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Croft, W. (2000). Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. London: Longman Linguistics Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, P. H., Provencher, L., Sloane, S., Dugatkin, L. A., Spohn, B., Rogers, L., & Alfieri, M. (1996). Evolving cooperation: The role of individual recognition. BioSystems, 37(1), 49–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Errico, F., García Moreno, R., & Rifkin, R. F. (2012). Technological, elemental and colorimetric analysis of an engraved ochre fragment from the Middle Stone Age levels of Klasies River Cave 1, South Africa. Journal of Archaeological Science, 39(4), 942–952.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1879). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (2nd edn). Edition used: The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. With an introduction by James Moore and Adrian Desmond. London: Penguin Books, 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake, E. (1992). Homo Aestheticus: Where art comes from and why. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake, E. (2007). What art is and what art does: An overview of contemporary evolutionary hypotheses. In C. Martindale, P. Locher, & V. M. Petrov (Eds.), Evolutionary and neurocognitive approaches to aesthetics, creativity, and the arts (pp. 1–14). Amityville: Baywood Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dor, D., & Jablonka, E. (2010). Plasticity and canalization in the evolution of linguistic communication: An evolutionary developmental approach. In R. K. Larson, V. Déprez, & H. Yamakid (Eds.), The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives (pp. 135–147). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin, L. A. (2002). Cooperation in animals: An evolutionary overview. Biology and Philosophy, 17(4), 459–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin, L. A., & Reeve, H. K. (1997). Cooperation among animals: An evolutionary perspective. Oxford: University Press Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6(5), 178–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton, D. (2009). The art instinct: Beauty, pleasure and human evolution. New York: Bloomsbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1988). The biological foundation of aesthetics. In I. Rentschler, B. Herzberger, & D. Epstein (Eds.), Beauty and the brain: Biological aspects of aesthetics (pp. 29–68). Basel: Birkhäuser Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Emler, N. (1990). A social psychology of reputation. European Review of Social Psychology, 1(1), 171–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Endler, J. A. (1993). Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B, 340(1292), 215–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finnegan, R. (2002). Communicating: The multiple modes of human interconnection. New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T., Huber, L., & Bugnyar, T. (2010). Social cognition and the evolution of language: Constructing cognitive phylogenies. Neuron, 65(6), 795–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble, C. (1999). The Palaeolithic societies of Europe. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (2004). Cooperation and the evolution of symbolic communication. In K. D. Oller & U. Griebel (Eds.), Evolution of communication systems: A comparative approach (pp. 237–256). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P., Brinck, I., & Osvath, M. (2012). The tripod effect: Co-evolution of cooperation, cognition and communication. In T. Schilhab, F. Stjernfelt, & T. Deacon (Eds.), The symbolic species evolved (pp. 193–222). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gombrich, E. H. (1995). The story of art. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall PTR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grammer, K., Fink, B., Moller, A. P., & Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics: Sexual selection and the biology of beauty. Biological Reviews, 78, 385–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayden, B. (2012). Neandertal social structure? Oxford Journal of Archaeology, 31(1), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C., D’Errico, F., Vanhaeren, M., van Niekerk, K., & Jacobs, Z. (2004). Middle Stone Age shell beads from south Africa. Science, 304(5669), 404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., D’Errico, F., & Watts, I. (2009). Engraved ochres from the Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 57(1), 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood, C. S., D’Errico, F., van Niekerk, K. L., Coquinot, Y., Jacobs, Z., Lauritzen, S.-E., Menu, M., & García-Moreno, R. (2011). A 100,000-year-old ochre-processing workshop at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Science, 334(6053), 219–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, R. A., & Dunbar, R. I. (2003). Social network size in humans. Human Nature, 14(1), 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, D. (2006). Understanding the origins of paleoart: The neurovisual resonance theory and brain functioning. Paleoanthropology, 2006, 54–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horan, R. D., Bulte, E., & Shogren, J. F. (2005). How trade saved humanity from biological exclusion: An economic theory of Neanderthal extinction. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 58(1), 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hovers, E., Shimon, I., Bar-Yosef, O., & Vandermeersch, B. (2003). An Early Case of Color Symbolism: Ochre Use by Modern Humans in Qafzeh Cave. Current Anthropology, 44(4), 491–522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 9(4), 156–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosse, K. (1990). Group size and societal complexity: Thresholds in the long-term memory. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 9(3), 275–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, S. L., & Stiner, M. C. (2007). Body ornamentation as information technology: Towards an understanding of the significance of early beads. In P. Mellars (Ed.), Rethinking the human revolution (pp. 45–54). Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1993). Gesture and speech. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A., & Vygotsky, L. S. (1992). Ape, primitive man, and child: Essays in the history of behavior. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mather, J. A. (2004). Cephalopod skin displays: From concealment to communication. In K. Oller & U. Griebel (Eds.), Evolution of communication systems: A comparative approach (pp. 193–214). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty, S., & Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn’t: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39(5), 453–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty, S., & Stringer, C. (2007). The coast in colour. Nature, 449(7164), 793–794.

    Google Scholar 

  • McElreath, R., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (2003). Shared norms can lead to the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology, 44(1), 122–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. London: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro Abadía, O., & González Morales, M. R. (2010). Redefining Neanderthals and Art: An Alternative Interpretation of the Multiple Species Model for the Origin of Behavioural Modernity. Oxford Journal of Archaeology, 29(3), 229–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naguib, M., & Riebel, K. (2006). Bird song: A key model in animal communication. Encyclopaedia of Language and Linguistics, 2, 40–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otte, D. (1974). Effects and functions in the evolution of signaling systems. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 5, 385–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (2005). Affective consciousness: Core emotional feelings in animals and humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(1), 30–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pokorny, J. J., & de Waal, F. B. (2009). Monkeys recognize the faces of group mates in photographs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(51), 21539–21543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Power, C. (1999). ‘Beauty Magic’: The origins of art. In R. Dunbar, C. Knight, & C. Power (Eds.), The evolution of culture (pp. 92–112). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riel-Salvatore, J., & Gravel-Miguel, C. (2013). Upper Palaeolithic mortuary practices in Eurasia: A critical look at the burial record. In S. Tarlow & L. Nilsson Stutz (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of the archaeology of death and burial (chapter 17). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semmann, D., Krambeck, H.-J., & Milinski, M. (2004). Strategic investment in reputation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 56(3), 248–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: A model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11(1), 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, C. (2009). Language as a biocultural niche and social institution. New Directions in Cognitive Linguistics, 24, 289–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. A. (2003). Human cooperation: Perspectives from behavioral ecology. In P. Hammerstein (Ed.), Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (pp. 401–428). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. J., & Leonard, W. R. (2009). Neanderthal energetics revisited: Insights into population dynamics and life history evolution. PaleoAnthropology, 2009, 220–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart-Fox, D., & Moussalli, A. (2008). Selection for social signalling drives the evolution of chameleon colour change. PLoS Biology, 6(1), e25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki, S., & Akiyama, E. (2005). Reputation and the evolution of cooperation in sizable groups. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272(1570), 1373–1377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Texier, P.-J., Porraz, G., Parkington, J., Rigaud, J.-P., Poggenpoel, C., Miller, C., Tribolo, C., Cartwright, C., Coudenneau, A., Klein, R., Steele, T., & Verna, C. (2010). A Howiesons Poort tradition of engraving ostrich eggshell containers dated to 60,000 years ago at Diepkloof Rock Shelter, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(14), 6180–6185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tilley, C. (1994). Interpreting material culture. In S. Pearce (Ed.), Interpreting objects and collections (pp. 67–75). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–691.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Post, D. J., & Hogeweg, P. (2008). Diet traditions and cumulative cultural processes as side-effects of grouping. Animal Behaviour, 75(1), 133–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanhaeren, M., D’Errico, F., Stringer, C., James, S. L., Todd, J. A., & Mienis, H. K. (2006). Middle Paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science, 312(5781), 1785–1788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanhaeren, M., d’Errico, F., van Niekerk, K. L., Henshilwood, C. S., & Erasmus, R. M. (2013). Thinking strings: Additional evidence for personal ornament use in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 64, 500–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verpooten, J., & Nelissen, M. (2010). Sensory exploitation and cultural transmission: The late emergence of iconic representations in human evolution. Theory in Biosciences, 129(2–3), 211–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, I. (1999). The origin of symbolic culture. In R. Dunbar, C. Knight, & C. Power (Eds.), The evolution of culture (pp. 113–146). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts, I. (2009). Red ochre, body painting, and language: Interpreting the Blombos ochre. In R. Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The cradle of language (pp. 62–92). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whallon, R. (2006). Social networks and information: Non-‘utilitarian’ mobility among hunter-gatherers. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 25(2), 259–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. (1992). Beyond art: Toward an understanding of the origins of material representation in Europe. Annual Review of Anthropology, 21, 537–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. (1993). Technological and social dimensions of ‘Aurignacian-age’ body ornaments across Europe. In H. Knecht, A. Pike-Tay, & R. White (Eds.), Before Lascaux: The complex record of the Early Upper Paleolithic (pp. 277–299). London: CRC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. (1983). Style and social information in Kalahari San projectile points. American Antiquity, 48(2), 253–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner, P. (1984). Reconsidering the behavioral basis for style: A case study among the Kalahari San. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 3(3), 190–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wobst, M. (1977). Stylistic behavior and information exchange. In C. E. Cleland (Ed.), For the director: Research essays in honor of James B. Griffin (pp. 317–342). Ann Arbor: Museum of Anthropology, University of Michigan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeki, S. (1999). Inner vision: An exploration of art and the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zilhão, J. (2007). The emergence of ornaments and art: An archaeological perspective on the origins of “Behavioral Modernity”. Journal of Archaeological Research, 15(1), 1–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zilhão, J. (2011). The emergence of language, art and symbolic thinking. In C. Henshilwood & F. D’Errico (Eds.), Homo symbolicus: The dawn of language, imagination and spirituality (pp. 111–131). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to the editors, E. Serrelli and F. Panebianco, for inviting me to contribute to this volume, and to two anonymous referees for their comments. This paper was completed within the project ‘Implementing the Extended Synthesis into the Sociocultural Domain’, directed by Nathalie Gontier and funded by the John Templeton Foundation, whom I thank for their support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Larissa Mendoza Straffon .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Mendoza Straffon, L. (2016). Signaling in Style: On Cooperation, Identity and the Origins of Visual Art. In: Panebianco, F., Serrelli, E. (eds) Understanding Cultural Traits. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24349-8_19

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-24349-8_19

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-24347-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-24349-8

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics