Skip to main content

Time in Intersubjectivity: Some Tools for Analysis

  • Chapter
Philosophy and Psychology of Time

Part of the book series: Studies in Brain and Mind ((SIBM,volume 9))

  • 966 Accesses

Abstract

Proper timing and other temporal factors are often viewed as important for rhythmic and synchronised social interaction. The chapter attempts to clarify what roles temporal properties play in interpersonal coordination. The tools for investigating the role of time in intersubjectivity are taken from Craver’s account of explanation, which I extend to intersubjective processes. A distinction is made between causal relevance, constitutive relevance, temporal constraints and background conditions. With the help of these tools, various cases of intersubjective coordination are scrutinised: the interaction between mother and infant and Thomas Fuchs’ phenomenological accounts of schizophrenia and depression, where the disturbance is supposed to involve both intersubjectivity and temporality. Thanks to these fine-grained distinctions we can give specific and different verdicts about the role of time in each particular case.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Of course, when pre-linguistic processes are analysed in terms of an analogy with language, as a kind of “protolanguage,” the irony is not big (see Halliday 1979).

  2. 2.

    Creating live music is often given as an example of a synchronised interaction. Not all members of the band are doing the same thing but they adjust their own playing to that of the others. This kind of mutual responsiveness is what is meant by synchrony here. Trevarthen (1999b, 157) takes this analogy a step further and writes about musicality as the innate “psycho-biological need in all humans.” See also Malloch and Trevarthen (2009).

  3. 3.

    This is not such a big break from more traditional approaches to causation as it may seem. Craver (2007, 95) notes that more usual causal relata such as events or processes can be easily replaced by variables as those can be understood in terms of their potential values (for example, an event can occur or fail to occur).

  4. 4.

    There is a question of whether reciprocity is real or illusory. Some have suggested that the illusion of reciprocal interaction comes from the fact that the mother adjusts her behaviour to infant’s spontaneous movements leaving the impression that the infant responds to mother (Papoušek and Papoušek 1995, 127). However, I examine the case under the assumption that it is as it seems. For a corroboration of Murray and Trevarthen’s results, see Nadel et al. (1999).

  5. 5.

    Fuchs (2013, 81) suggests that this range is 200–800 ms. He refers to Papoušek and Papoušek (1995), although I did not manage to find such data in that particular chapter. Notwithstanding this, a range in such a vicinity seems plausible.

  6. 6.

    Since the clinical aspects are not my main focus, I just proceed from Fuchs’ notions of schizophrenia and depression, without attempting to compare them to standard classifications such as DSM or criticizing his approach. For a recent critique of Fuchs, see Ratcliffe (2012) who argues that depression is more varied than Fuchs conceives it.

References

  • Brazelton, T. Berry, Barbara Koslowski, and Mary Main. 1974. The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction. In The effect of the infant on its caregiver, ed. M. Lewis and L.A. Rosenblum, 49–76. London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craver, Carl. 2007. Explaining the brain: Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dainton, Barry. 2006. Stream of consciousness: Unity and continuity in conscious experience, 2nd ed. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, Hanne, Ezequiel Di Paolo, and Shaun Gallagher. 2010. Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(10): 441–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Droit-Volet, Sylvie, and Sandrine Gil. 2009. The time-emotion paradox. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 364: 1943–1953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Droit-Volet, Sylvie, Sophie Fayolle, Mathilde Lamotte, and Sandrine Gil. 2013. Time, emotion and the embodiment of timing. Timing & Time Perception 1: 99–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, Thomas. 2001. Melancholia as a desynchronization: Towards a psychopathology of interpersonal time. Psychopathology 34: 179–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, Thomas. 2005. Implicit and explicit temporality. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology 8: 323–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, Thomas. 2007. The temporal structure of intentionality and its disturbance in schizophrenia. Psychopathology 40: 229–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, Thomas. 2013. Temporality and psychopathology. Phenomenology and Cognitive Sciences 12: 75–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, Shaun. 1998. The inordinance of time. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, Shaun, and Daniel D. Hutto. 2008. Understanding others through primary interaction and narrative practice. In The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity, ed. J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, and E. Itkonen, 17–38. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gratier, Maya, and Gisèle Apter-Danon. 2009. The improvised musicality of belonging: Repetition and variation in mother-infant vocal interaction. In Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, ed. Stephen Malloch and Colwyn Trevarthen, 301–327. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, Michael A.K. 1979. One child’s protolanguage. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. Margaret Bullowa, 171–190. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, Edmund. 1991. On the phenomenology of the consciousness of internal time (1893–1917). Trans. J. B. Brough. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd, Dan. 2012. Neural correlates of temporality: Default mode variability and temporal awareness. Consciousness and Cognition 21(2): 695–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malloch, Stephen, and Colwyn Trevarthen (eds.). 2009. Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, Lynne, and Colwyn Trevarthen. 1985. Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Social perception in infants, ed. T.M. Field and N.A. Fox, 177–197. Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, Jacqueline, Isabelle Carchon, Claude Kervella, Daniel Marcelli, and Denis Réserbat-Plantey. 1999. Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science 2(2): 164–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek, Hanuš, and Mechthild Papoušek. 1995. Intuitive parenting. In Handbook of parenting: Biology and ecology of parenting, vol. 2, ed. Marc H. Bornstein, 117–136. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piccinini, Gualtiero, and Carl Craver. 2011. Integrating psychology and neuroscience: Functional analyses as mechanism sketches. Synthese 183: 283–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliffe, Matthew. 2012. Varieties of temporal experience in depression. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 37: 114–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodemeyer, Lanei M. 2006. Intersubjective temporality: It’s about time. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, Joanna. 1974. Early language development: Towards a communicational analysis. In The integration of a child into a social world, ed. M.P.M. Richards, 185–213. London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spaulding, Shannon. 2012. Introduction to debates on embodied social cognition. Phenomenology and Cognitive Sciences 11: 431–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before speech: The beginning of human communication, ed. Margaret Bullowa, 321–347. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1984. How control of movements develops. In Human motor actions: Bernstein reassessed, ed. H.T.A. Whiting, 223–261. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1986. Development of intersubjective motor control in infants. In Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control, ed. M.G. Wade and H.T.A. Whiting, 209–261. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhof.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1999a. Intersubjectivity. In The MIT encyclopedia of cognitive sciences, ed. R.A. Wilson and F.C. Keil, 415–419. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, Colwyn. 1999b. Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae 3(1): 157–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2008. Shared minds and the science of fiction: Why theories will differ. In The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity, ed. J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, and E. Itkonen, vii–xii. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, Colwyn, and Penelope Hubley. 1978. Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language, ed. Andrew Lock, 183–229. London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, James. 2003. Making things happen. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, James, and Christopher Hitchcock. 2003. Explanatory generalizations, part I: A counterfactual account. Noûs 37(1): 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, Dan. 2003. Husserl’s phenomenology. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, Dan. 2005. Subjectivity and selfhood: Investigating the first-person perspective. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Work on this chapter was supported by the COST Action TD0904 (Time in Mental Activity), institutional research funding IUT20-5 of the Estonian Ministry of Education and Research and a grant ETF9117 from the Estonian Science Foundation. I am grateful to Colwyn Trevarthen for reading suggestions and to Alexander Davies for checking my English.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bruno Mölder .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Mölder, B. (2016). Time in Intersubjectivity: Some Tools for Analysis. In: Mölder, B., Arstila, V., Øhrstrøm, P. (eds) Philosophy and Psychology of Time. Studies in Brain and Mind, vol 9. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-22195-3_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics