Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 11))

Abstract

The goal of this study is to examine the role of syntactic units of written production by analysing two chronometric measures during text production: pause length and writing rate. Children and adolescents (9–10-, 12–13- and 15–16-year-olds, 40 in each group) were asked to produce narrative and expository texts in both the written and spoken modality. The current analysis focuses exclusively on the written texts, but will examine the effect of the order of production: half of the children in each age group produced the written texts first and then the spoken texts, while the other half produced the spoken texts first and then the written texts. The written texts were collected using a digitizing tablet with the Eye&Pen software (Chesnet and Alamargot. Année Psychol 105(3):477–520, 2005). We consider that long pauses reflect planning of a unit and that fast writing speed reflects that planning is completed. Because our specific interest concerns syntactic units, the texts were coded for syntactic connectivity between propositions, including juxtaposition, coordination, finite and non-finite subordination, and center-embedding. Results show very strong developmental differences in planning practices as measured by the dynamics of production (pause length and writing speed), with the length of the fluently produced unit growing in size. The youngest subjects show evidence of planning at the clause level, while the older subjects show evidence of planning at the level of the propositional package. Results also show differences in the writing dynamics between the expository and narrative texts, revealing that, the expository texts are more challenging for some of our young writers. For some writers, in particular the 7th graders the written texts produced in the order spoken then written are produced more fluently than those produced in the order written then spoken.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The examples are identified in the following way: grade level (G5, G7, G9), subject number, sex (f = female, m = male), text type (n = narrative, e = expository), order (written1 = written first, written2 = written second).

References

  • Aparici, M. (2010). El desarrollo de la conectividad discursiva en diferentes géneros y modalidades de producción. Doctoral dissertation, Universidad de Barcelona, Spain

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. London: L. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2004a). Between emergence and mastery: The long developmental route of language acquisition. In R. A. Berman (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence (Trends in Language Acquisition Research (TILAR), Vol. 3, pp. 9–34). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A. (2004b). Introduction : Developing discourse stance in different text types and languages. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 105–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Nir-Sagiv, B. (2009). Clause-packaging in narratives: A crosslinguistic developmental study. In J. Guo, E. Lieven, S. Ervin-Tripp, N. Budwig, S. Özçalişkan, & K. Nakamura (Eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the psychology of language: Research in the tradition of Dan I. Slobin (pp. 149–162). New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Ravid, D. (2009). Becoming a literate language user: Oral and written text construction across adolescence. In D. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of literacy (pp. 92–111). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. A., & Verhoeven, L. (2002). Cross-linguistic perspectives on the development of text-production abilities: speech and writing. Written Language and Literacy, 5, 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Swanson, H. L. (1994). Modifying Hayes and Flower’s model of skilled writing to explain beginning and developing writing. In E. C. Butterfield (Ed.),Children’s writing : Toward a process theory of the development of skilled writing (pp. 57–81). London/Greenwich: JAI Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdin, B., Cogis, D., & Foulin, J. N. (2010). Influence des traitements graphomoteurs et orthographiques sur la production de textes écrits : perspective pluridisciplinaire. Langages, 177(1), 57–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chanquoy, L., Foulin, J. N., & Fayol, M. (1990). Temporal management of short text writing by children and adults. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 10(5), 513–540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chesnet, D., & Alamargot, D. (2005). Analyse en temps réel des activités oculaires et grapho-motrices du scripteur: intérêt du dispositif « Eye and Pen ». L'Année Psychologique, 105(3), 477–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing. College Composition and Communication, 32(4), 365–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foster, P., Tonkyn, A., & Wigglesworth, G. (2000). Measuring spoken language: A unit for all reasons. Applied Linguistics, 21(3), 354–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foulin, J. N. (1998). To what extent does pause location predict pause duration in adults’ and children’s writing? Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 17(3), 601–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gayraud, F., Jisa, H., & Viguié, A. (1999). The development of syntactic packaging in French children’s written and spoken texts. Developing Literacy Across Genres, Modalities, and Languages, 1, 169–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gayraud, F., & Martinie, B. (2004). Complexité syntaxique et difficulté de traitement: l’exemple des relatives. In A. Rabatel (Ed.), Interactions orales en contexte didactique. Mieux (se) comprendre pour mieux (se) parler et mieux (s’)apprendre. Lyon: PUL.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R. (2012). Modeling and remodeling writing. Written Communication, 29(3), 369–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P., & Thompson, S. A. (2008). Projectability and clause combining in interaction. In R. Laury (Ed.), Crosslinguistic studies of clause combining: The multifunctionality of conjunctions (pp. 99–124). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, K. W. (1965). Grammatical structures written at three grade levels. In National Council of Teachers of English, III.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, K. W. (1970). Syntactic maturity in schoolchildren and adults. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 35, 1–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Immonen, S., & Mäkisalo, J. (2010). Pauses reflecting the processing of syntactic units in monolingual text production and translation. Hermes–Journal of Language and Communication Studies, 44, 45–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jisa, H. (2000). Increasing cohesion in narratives: A developmental study of maintaining and reintroducing subjects in French. Linguistics, 38(3), 591–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jisa, H., & Viguié, A. (2005). A developmental perspective on the role of on in written and spoken expository texts in French. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 125–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maggio, S., Lété, B., Chenu, F., Jisa, H., & Fayol, M. (2012). Tracking the mind during writing: immediacy, delayed, and anticipatory effects on pauses and writing rate. Reading and Writing, 25(9), 2131–2151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuhashi, A. (1982). Explorations in the real-time production of written discourse. In M. Nystrand (Ed.), What writers know: The language, process, and structure of written discourse (pp. 269–290). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthiessen, C., & Thompson, S. A. (1988). The structure of discourse and ‘Subordination’. In J. Haiman & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Clause combining in grammar and discourse (pp. 275–329). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur-Palandre, A. (2008). Later language development: Syntactic packaging in written and spoken French. In Proceedings of the second Oxford postgraduate conference, Oxford, 21–22 September, 2007 (pp. 173–180). Oxford: Faculty of Linguistics, Philology, and Phonetics, University of Oxford. Weblink: http://www.ling-phil.ox.ac.uk/events/lingo/papers/Proceedings.pdf

  • Mazur-Palandre, A. (2015). Overcoming preferred argument structure in written French: Development, modality, text type. Written Language and Literacy, 18, 25–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational Psychology Review, 8(3), 299–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nir-Sagiv, B. (2008). Clause packages as constructions in developing narrative discourse. Doctoral dissertation, Tel Aviv University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nir, B., & Berman, R. A. (2010). Complex syntax as a window on contrastive rhetoric. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 744–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olive, T., Alves, R. A., & Castro, S. L. (2009a). Cognitive processes in writing during pause and execution periods. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21(5), 758–785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olive, T., Favart, M., Beauvais, C., & Beauvais, L. (2009b). Children’s cognitive effort and fluency in writing: Effects of genre and of handwriting automatisation. Learning and Instruction, 19, 229–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paolacci, V., & Rossi-Gensane, N. (2012). Quelles images de la phrase dans les écrits d’élèves de fin d’école primaire française ? Description linguistique et réponses didactiques aux difficultés des élèves. SHS Web of Conferences, 1, 341–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & van Gompel, R. P. G. (2006). Syntactic parsing. In M. J. Trawler & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 455–503). Amsterdam: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Piolat, A. (1983). Localisation syntaxique des pauses et planification dans le discours. L’Année Psychologique, 83, 377–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schilperoord, J. (2002). On the cognitive status of pauses in discourse production. In T. Olive & M. Levy (Eds.), Contemporary tools and techniques for studying writing (pp. 61–87). Dordrecht: Academic Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schilperoord, J., & Sanders, T. (1999). How hierarchical text structure affects retrieval processes: Implications of pause and text analysis. In Knowing what to write. Conceptual processes in text production (pp. 13–33). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, S. A., & Haiman, J. (1988). Clause combining in grammar and discourse. Amsterdam: John Benjamin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Hell, J. G., Verhoeven, L., & van Bijsterverldt, L. M. (2008). Pause time patterns in writing narrative and expository texts by children and adults. Discourse Processes, 45, 406–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgment

The authors wish to express their gratitude to the anonymous reviewers for their very helpful comments and suggestions. They are also grateful to the LABEX ASLAN (ANR-10-LABX-0081) of Université de Lyon for its support within the French program “Investissements d’Avenir” (ANR-11-IDEX-0007) operated by the National Research Agency (ANR).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Emilie Ailhaud .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ailhaud, E., Chenu, F., Jisa, H. (2016). A Developmental Perspective on the Units of Written French. In: Perera, J., Aparici, M., Rosado, E., Salas, N. (eds) Written and Spoken Language Development across the Lifespan. Literacy Studies, vol 11. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-21136-7_17

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-21136-7_17

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-21135-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-21136-7

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics