Skip to main content

Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Identity

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Psychology

Part of the book series: Evolutionary Psychology ((EVOLPSYCH))

Abstract

A complete understanding of the psychology of social identity requires not only descriptions of how social identification processes work but also an account of why the underlying psychological mechanisms have evolved. This chapter focuses on the evolution of coalitional (or “tribal”) social identity (i.e., the type of social identity associated with nationality, ethnicity, religion, and class). Coalitional social identity appears to involve a readiness to incur costs for the collective, which may yield cooperative benefits. However, it has not been obvious why reaping the benefits of intragroup cooperation would be facilitated by social identification processes. We suggest that social identity may be related to the signaling of coalitional membership and cooperative intent. Specifically, we argue that social identity may constitute a self-represented summary of the loyalty-signaling characteristics that one has acquired. Based on this hypothesized ultimate function of social identity, we derive predictions regarding the proximate psychology of social identity. We suggest that further research may examine whether social identity involves private social identities (for balancing costs and benefits of group membership) and public social identities (for strategically influencing the behavior of others).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    For some, it might seem obvious that the function of an automobile is locomotion. However, for a naïve observer, the function may not be obvious at all. The observer may note that an automobile burns fuel, disrupts airflow, makes noise, and sometimes causes accidents, and take any one of these to be its function. Obviously, different conjectures regarding function (e.g., locomotion vs. fuel burning) yield different hypotheses regarding form.

  2. 2.

    There are several influential perspectives that are relevant to social identity and intergroup psychology. One notable perspective is terror management theory (TMT), which explains many human psychological phenomena—including intergroup bias—as resulting from motivations to uphold cultural worldviews (which, in turn, exist to assuage anxieties about death). Interestingly, evolutionary psychologists have attempted to explain many of the TMT-related phenomena as manifestations of coalitional psychology (e.g., Navarrete and Fessler 2005). Thus, this is another example of an explanation relying on intrapsychic needs being updated by a more contemporary evolutionary psychological perspective.

References

  • Aarøe, L., & Petersen, M. B. (2013). Hunger games: Fluctuations in blood glucose levels influence support for social welfare. Psychological Science, 24, 2550–2556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.). (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, H. C., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in cognition: Framing the debate. Psychological Review, 113, 628–647.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest. London: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boelkins, R. C., & Wilson, A. P. (1972). Intergroup social dynamics of the Cayo Santiago rhesus ( Macaca mulatta) with special reference to changes in group membership by males. Primates, 13, 125–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S. (2009). Did warfare among ancestral hunter-gatherers affect the evolution of human social behaviors? Science, 324, 1293–1298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2009). Culture and the evolution of human cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 3281–3288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love or outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55, 429–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B. (2003). Optimal distinctiveness, social identity, and the self. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. 480–491). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B., & Caporael, L. R. (2006). An evolutionary perspective on social identity: Revisiting groups. In M. Schaller, J. A. Simpson, & D. T. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 143–162). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brubaker, R. (2002). Ethnicity without groups. Archives Européenes de Sociologie, 43, 163–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchan, J. C., Alberts, S. C., Silk, J. B., & Altmann, J. (2003). True paternal care in a multi-male primate society. Nature, 425, 179–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (Ed.). (2005). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2013). Evolutionary psychology: New perspectives on cognition and motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 201–229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Kurzban, R. (2003). Perceptions of race. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 173–179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2013). A solution to the mysteries of morality. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 477–496.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. I. M., & Barrett, L. (Eds.). (2007). The Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Duncan, L. A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 7, 333–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 1–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, D. L. (2001). The deadly ethnic riot. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawanaka, K. (1973). Intertroop relationships among Japanese monkeys. Primates, 14, 113–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinzler, K. D., Shutts, K., & Correll, J. (2010). Priorities in social categories. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 581–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., & Aktipis, C. A. (2007). Modularity and the social mind: Are psychologists too self-ish? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 131–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 187–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 15387–15392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leary, M. R. (2012). Sociometer theory. In L. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 141–159). Los Angeles: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 67–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Navarrete, C. D., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2005). Normative bias and adaptive challenges: A relational approach to coalitional psychology and a critique of terror management theory. Evolutionary Psychology, 3, 297–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navarrete, C. D., McDonald, M. M., Molina, L. E., & Sidanius, J. (2010). Prejudice at the nexus of race and gender: An outgroup male target hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 933–945.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J. H. (2012). Evolutionary perspectives on intergroup prejudice: Implications for promoting tolerance. In S. C. Roberts (Ed.), Applied evolutionary psychology (pp. 186–200). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature: The decline of violence in history and its causes. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reicher, S. D., Haslam, S. A., Spears, R., & Reynolds, K. J. (2012). A social mind: The context of John Turner’s work and its influence. European Review of Social Psychology, 23, 344–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Faulkner, J. (2003). Prehistoric dangers and contemporary prejudices. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 105–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (1997). The symbolic self in evolutionary context. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 80–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, P. W. (1977). Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science, 197, 1246–1253.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33–47). Monterey: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todrank, J., Heth, G., & Johnston, R. E. (1998). Kin recognition in golden hamsters: Evidence for kinship odours. Animal Behaviour, 55, 377–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Vugt, M., & Hart, C. M. (2004). Social identity as social glue: The origins of group loyalty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 585–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Vugt, M., & Park, J. H. (2010). The tribal instinct hypothesis: Evolution and the social psychology of intergroup relations. In S. Sturmer & M. Snyder (Eds.), The psychology of prosocial behavior: Group processes, intergroup relations, and helping (pp. 13–32). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Vugt, M., De Cremer, D., & Janssen, D. P. (2007). Gender differences in cooperation and competition: The male-warrior hypothesis. Psychological Science, 18, 19–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Von Hippel, W., & Trivers, R. (2011). The evolution and psychology of self-deception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 1–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winegard, B., & Deaner, R. O. (2010). The evolutionary significance of Red Sox nation: Sports fandom as a by-product of coalitional psychology. Evolutionary Psychology, 8, 432–446.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., Fischer, R., Reddish, P., Skewes, J., Geertz, A. W., Roepstorff, A., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Extreme rituals promote prosociality. Psychological Science, 24, 1602–1605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Pascal Boyer for his helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Justin H. Park Ph.D. .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Park, J., van Leeuwen, F. (2015). Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Identity. In: Zeigler-Hill, V., Welling, L., Shackelford, T. (eds) Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Psychology. Evolutionary Psychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-12697-5_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics