Skip to main content

The Significance of Visual Representations in the Teaching of Science

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Science Teachers’ Use of Visual Representations

Part of the book series: Models and Modeling in Science Education ((MMSE,volume 8))

Abstract

The natural world is highly dynamic and complex. Scientists aspire to understand this complex world through observations, investigations and inferences. For this purpose, scientists isolate specific phenomena for studying and examine its features through its simplified models and visual representations (VRs). The constructed scientific knowledge is then communicated to the science community through various modalities like, text and image. Socializing students into the world of science therefore, requires educators among other goals, to teach students all about models and representations, to expose students to these representations diversity and characteristics, to use them for promoting the understanding of phenomena and to develop students’ ability to think with representations as scientist do. Teachers’ task though, is not an easy one, because scientific phenomena and its representations are difficult to grasp; they are highly complex, comprising many components, micro and macro levels with explicit or implicit interactions within and among them, they are concrete or abstract, or are dynamic or static entities. In addition, to develop students’ representational competencies teachers themselves have to be fluent, proficient and efficient in these representations use, develop pedagogical-visual-content-knowledge for teaching with visual representations, be aware of the difficulties inherent to the use of representations or their generation, or be able to identify student-related difficulties, those hindering their learning. Because visual representations are widely used to support science teaching, meta representational competence should be developed. However, this need remains an untreated goal, and researchers report students difficulties to learn with visual representations. The chapter discusses the difficulties involved in teaching and how visual representations may this goal.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abell, S. K., & Roth, M. (1995). Reflections on a 5th grade science lesson: Making sense of children’s understanding of scientific models. International Journal of Science Education, 17, 59–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, S. E. (2006). DeFT: A conceptual framework for considering learning representations. Learning and Instruction, 16, 183–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth, S. E. (2008). How do animations influence learning? In H. Robinson & G. Schraw (Eds.), Recent innovations in educational technology that facilitate student learning (pp. 37–67). Charlotte: Information Age.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anagün, Ş. S., & İşcen, C. F. (2010). Pre-service teachers’ perceptions and preferences about visualization. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2, 2227–2232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ayers, P., & Sweller, J. (2005). The split-attention principle in multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 135–146). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bodemer, D., Ploetzner, R., Feueelein, I., & Spada, H. (2004). The active integration of information during learning with dynamic and interactive visualizations. Learning and Instruction, 14, 325–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booth Sweeney, L., & Sterman, J. D. (2007). Thinking about systems: Student and teacher conceptions of natural and social systems. System Dynamics Review, 23(2/3), 285–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowen, G. M., & Roth, W. M. (2002). Why students may not learn to interpret scientific inscriptions? Research in Science Education, 32, 303–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school. Washington, DC: National Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brookshire, J., Scarff, L. F. V., & Moses, L. E. (2002). The influence of illustrations on children’s book preferences and comprehension. Reading Psychology, 23(4), 323–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butcher, K. R. (2006). Learning from text with diagrams: Promoting mental model development and inference generation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 182–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carney, N. R., & Levin, R. J. (2002). Pictorial illustrations still improve students’ learning from text. Educational Psychology Review, 14(1), 5–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H. (2005). Common sense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14, 161–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Slotta, J. D., & de Leeuw, N. (1994). From things to processes: A theory of conceptual change for learning science concepts. Learning and Instruction, 4, 27–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (2000). Model-based learning as a key research area for science education. International Journal of Science Education, 22(9), 1041–1053.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colin, P., Chauvet, F., & Viennot, L. (2002). Reading images in optics: Students’ difficulties and teachers’ views. International Journal of Science Education, 24(3), 313–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coll, R. K., & Taylor, I. (2005). The role of models and analogies in science education. International Journal of Science Education, 27, 183–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, M. (2011). Teachers’ use of visual representations in the science classroom. Science Education International, 22(3), 175–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, B. A., Zembal-Saul, C., Munford, D., & Friedrichsen, P. (2005). Confronting prospective teachers’ ideas of evolution and scientific inquiry using technology and inquiry-based tasks. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 42(6), 613–637.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Bock, D., Verschaffel, L., Janssen, D., VanDooren, W., & Claes, K. (2003). Do realistic contexts and graphical representations always have a beneficial impact on students’ performance? Negative evidence from a study on modeling non-linear geometry problems. Learning and Instruction, 13, 441–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, T., & van Joolingen, W. R. (1998). Scientific discovery learning with computer simulations of conceptual domains. Review of Educational Research, 68(2), 179–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, T., Ainsworth, S., Donson, M., van der Hultst, A., Levonen, J., et al. (1998). Acquiring knowledge in science and mathematics: The use of multiple representations in technology-based learning environments. In M. W. van Someren, P. Reiman, H. P. A. Boshuizen, & T. de Jong (Eds.), Learning with multiple representations (pp. 9–40). Oxford: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, T., Beishuizen, J., Hulshof, C., Prins, F., van Rijn, H., & van Somersen, M. (2005). Determinants of discovery learning in a complex situation learning environment. In P. Gardenfors & P. Johansson (Eds.), Cognition, education and communication technology (pp. 257–283). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • deVries, E., Demetriadis, S., & Ainsworth, S. (2009). External representations for learning. In N. Balacheff, S. Ludvigsen, T. de Jong, A. Lazonder, S. Barnes, & L. Mondandon (Eds.), Technology enhanced learning: Principles and products (pp. 137–154). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, 105–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A., & Sherin, B. L. (2000). Meta-representation: An introduction. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 19, 385–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eilam, B. (2004). Drops of water and of soap solution: Students’ constraining mental models of the nature of matter. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 41(10), 970–993.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eilam, B. (2012a). Teaching, learning, and visual literacy: The dual role of visual representation in the teaching profession. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eilam, B. (2012b). System thinking and feeding relations: Learning with a live ecosystem model. Instructional Science, 40(2), 213–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eilam, B. (2012c). Chapter 4: Possible constraints of visualization in biology: Challenges in learning with multi-media. In D. Treagust & C.-Y. Tsui (Eds.), Multiple representations in biological education (The series of models and modeling in science education, pp. 55–73). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eilam, B., & Poyas, Y. (2008). Learning with multiple representations: Extending multimedia learning beyond the lab. Learning and Instruction, 18(4), 368–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elia, I., & Philippou, G. (2004). The functions of pictures in problem solving. Proceedings of the 28th Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, 2, 327–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elia, I., Gagatsis, A., & Demetriou, A. (2007). The effects of different modes of representation on the solution of one-step additive problems. Learning and Instruction, 17, 658–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eshach, H., & Schwartz, J. L. (2002). Understanding children’s comprehension of visual displays of complex information. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 11(4), 333–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, J. (1993). Models and modelling in science education. Hatfield: Association of Science Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, J. K. (2007). Visualization: A metacognitive skill in science and science education. In J. K. Gilbert (Ed.), Visualization in science education (pp. 9–27). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, J., & Boulter, C. (Eds.). (2000). Developing models in science education. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginns, P. (2006). Integrating information: A meta-analysis of the spatial contiguity and temporal contiguity effects. Learning and Instruction, 16, 511–525.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grotzer, T. (2005). The role of complex causal models in students’ understanding of science. Studies in Science Education, 41, 117–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grotzer, T. A., & Bell Baska, B. (2003). How does grasping the underlying causal structures of ecosystems impact students’ understanding? Journal of Biological Education, 38(1), 16–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, A. G., & Treagust, D. F. (2000). Learning about atoms, molecules and chemical bonds: A case-study of multiple model use in grade-11 chemistry. Science Education, 84, 352–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G., & Inagaki, K. (1994). Young children’s naive theory of biology. Cognition, 50, 171–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M., & Just, M. A. (1989). Understanding machines from text and diagrams. In H. Mandl & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Knowledge acquisition from text and pictures (pp. 171–194). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegarty, M., Narayanan, N. H., & Freitas, P. (2002). Understanding machines from multimedia and hypermedia presentations. In J. Otero, A. C. Greaser, & J. Leon (Eds.), The psychology of science text comprehension (pp. 357–384). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heiser, J., & Tversky, B. (2006). Arrows in comprehending and producing mechanical diagrams. Cognitive Science, 30(3), 581–592.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hmelo-Silver, C. E. (2004). Problem-based learning: What and how students learn? Educational Psychology Review, 16, 235–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hmelo-Silver, C. E., & Azevedo, R. A. (2006). Understanding complex systems: Some core challenges. Journal of the Learning Science, 15, 53–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hmelo-Silver, C. E., & Pfeffer, M. G. (2004). Comparing experts and novice understanding of a complex system from the perspective of structures, behaviors, and functions. Cognitive Science, 28, 127–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hmelo-Silver, C. E., Marathe, S., & Liu, L. (2007a). Fish swim, rocks sit, and lungs breathe: Expert-novice understanding of complex systems. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 16, 307–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hmelo-Silver, C. E., Liu, L., Gray, S., Finkelstein, H., & Schwartz, R. (2007b). Enacting things differently: Using NetLogo models to learn about complex systems. Paper presented at the Biennial meeting of European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction. Hungary: Budapest.

    Google Scholar 

  • Höffler, T. N., & Leutner, D. (2007). Instructional animation versus static pictures: A meta-analysis. Learning and Instruction, 17, 722–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, K., & Thomas, D. (2001). Cognitive comparisons of students’ systems modeling in ecology. Journal of Education and Technology, 10(4), 319–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, M. J. (2001). Problem solving, cognition, and complex systems: Differences between experts and novices. Complexity, 6(3), 41–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobson, M. J., & Wilensky, U. (2006). Complex systems in education: Scientific and educational importance and implications for the learning sciences. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(1), 11–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. G., & Taylor, A. R. (2009). Developing a sense of scale: Looking backward. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 46(4), 460–475.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. G., Tretter, T., Taylor, A., & Oppewal, T. (2008). Experienced and novice teachers’ concepts of spatial scale. International Journal of Science Education, 30(3), 409–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1992). Chapter 4: The origin of an autonomous biology. In M. R. Gunnar & M. Maratsos (Eds.), Modularity and constraints in language and cognition (The Minnesota symposia on child psychology, Vol. 25, pp. 103–137). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klopfer, E. (2003). Technologies to support the creation of complex systems models – Using StarLogo software with students. Biosystems, 71(1–2), 111–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (2006). Graph design for the eye and mind. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (2005). Education for thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Why a diagram is (sometimes) worth 10000 words. Cognitive Science, 11(1), 65–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leach, J., Driver, R., Scott, P., & Wood-Robinson, C. (1996). Children’s ideas about ecology: Vol. 2. Ideas found in children aged 5–16 about the cycling of matter. International Journal of Science Education, 18, 19–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, O., Eichinger, C. D., Anderson, W. C., Berkheimer, D. G., & Blakeslee, D. T. (1990). Changing middle school students’ conceptions of matter and molecules. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 30, 249–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J. R., & Mayer, R. E. (1993). Understanding illustrations in texts. In B. K. Britton, A. Woodward, & M. Binkley (Eds.), Learning from textbooks: Theory and practice (pp. 95–113). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J. R., Shriberg, L. K., & Berry, J. K. (1983). A concrete strategy for remembering abstract prose. American Educational Research Journal, 20(2), 277–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, S. T., & Wilensky, U. (2008). Inventing a “mid level” to make ends meet: Reasoning between levels of complexity. Cognition and Instruction, 26, 1–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewalter, D. (2003). Cognitive strategies for learning from static and dynamic visuals. Learning and Instruction, 13, 177–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, X., & Lesniak, K. (2006). Progression in children’s understanding of the matter concept from elementary to high school. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43(3), 320–347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, R. K. (2003). Animation and learning: Selective processing of information in dynamic graphics. Learning and Instruction, 13, 157–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (Ed.). (2005a). Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E. (2005b). Principles for managing essential processing in multimedia learning: Segmenting, pretraining, and modality principles. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (pp. 169–182). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., Hegarty, M., Mayer, S., & Campbell, J. (2005). When static media promote active learning: Annotated illustrations versus narrated animations in multimedia. The Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11(4), 256–265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michel, E., Roebers, C. M., & Schneider, W. (2007). Educational films in the classrooms: Increasing the benefit. Learning and Instruction, 17, 172–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moreno, R., & Valdez, A. (2005). Cognitive load and learning effects of having students organize pictures and words in multimedia environments: The role of student interactivity and feedback. Educational Technology Research and Development, 53(3), 35–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakhleh, M. B., & Samarapungavan, A. (1999). Elementary school children’s beliefs about matter. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36(7), 777–805.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakhleh, M. B., Samarapungavan, A., & Saglam, Y. (2005). Middle school students’ beliefs about matter. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 42(5), 581–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novick, S., & Nussbaum, J. (1981). Pupils’ understanding of the particulate nature of matter: A cross-age study. Science Education, 65(2), 187–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, O. C., & Hopkins, R. (1993). Instructional conditions for using dynamic visual displays: A review. Instructional Science, 21, 227–449.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peeck, J. (1987). The role of illustrations in processing and remembering illustrated text. In D. M. Willows & H. A. Houghton (Eds.), The psychology of illustration (Basic research, Vol. 1, pp. 115–151). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penner, D. E. (2000). Explaining systems: Investigating middle school students’ understanding of emergent phenomena. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(8), 784–806.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penner, D. E. (2001). Cognition, computers, and synthetic science: Building knowledge and meaning through modeling. Review of Research in Education, 25, 1–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettersson, R. (2009). Visual literacy and message design. TechTrends, 53(2), 38–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prain, V., & Waldrip, B. (2006). An exploratory study of teachers’ and students’ use of multi-modal representations of concepts in primary science. International Journal of Science Education, 28(15), 1843–1866.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prain, V., & Waldrip, B. (2008). A study of teachers’ perspectives about using multimodal representations of concepts to enhance science learning. Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics, and Technology Education, 8(1), 5–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, M. (1996). Beyond the centralized mindset. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 5(1), 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieber, L. P., & Kini, A. S. (1991). Theoretical foundations of instructional applications of computer-generated animated visuals. Journal of Computer-Based Instruction, 18, 83–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabelli, N. H. (2006). Complexity, technology, science, and education. Journal of the Learning Science, 15, 5–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schnotz, W., & Rasch, T. (2005). Enabling, facilitating and inhibiting effect of animations in multimedia learning: Why reducing of cognitive load can have negative results on learning. Educational Technology Research and Development, 53(3), 47–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schroeder, S., Richter, T., McElvany, N., Hachfeld, A., Baumert, J., Schnotz, W., Horz, H., & Ullrich, M. (2011). Teachers’ beliefs, instructional behaviors, and students’ engagement in learning from texts with instructional pictures. Learning and Instruction, 21, 403–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwan, S., & Riempp, R. (2004). The cognitive benefit of interactive videos: Learning to tie nautical knots. Learning and Instruction, 14, 293–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, L. S. (1986). Those who understand: Knowledge growth in teaching. Educational Researcher, 15, 4–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slotta, J. D., & Chi, M. T. H. (2006). Helping students understand challenging topics in science through ontology training. Cognition and Instruction, 24(20), 261–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slotta, D. J., Chi, M. T. H., & Joram, E. (1995). Assessing students’ misconceptions of physics concepts: An ontological basis for conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction, 13(3), 373–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweeney, B. L., & Sterman, J. D. (2007). Thinking about systems: Student and teacher conceptions of natural and social systems. System Dynamics Review, 23(2/3), 285–312.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tufte, E. R. (1997). Visual explanations: Images and quantities, evidence and narrative. Cheshire: Graphic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., Bauer-Morrison, J., & Bétrancourt, M. (2002). Animation: Can it facilitate? International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 57, 247–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeff, R. P., Waarlo, A. J., & Boersma, K. T. (2008). Systems modeling and the development of coherent understanding of cell biology. International Journal of Science Education, 30, 543–568.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vick, J. E., Azevedo, R., & Hofman, N. S. (2005, April). Expert-novice differences in the understanding and detection of self-regulated learning with hypermedia. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Montreal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (1994). Capturing and modeling the process of conceptual change. Learning and Instruction, 4, 45–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vosniadou, S. (2010). Instructional considerations in the use of external representations. The distinction between perceptually based depictions and pictures that represent conceptual models. In L. Verschaffel, E. De Corte, T. de Jong, & J. Elen (Eds.), Use of representations in reasoning and problem solving. Analysis and improvement (pp. 36–54). London/New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weidenmann, B. (1989). When god pictures fail: An information-processing approach to the effect of illustrations. In H. Mandl & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Knowledge acquisition from text and pictures (pp. 157–171). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (1992). Cognitive development: Foundational theories of core domains. Annual Review of Psychology, 43, 337–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilensky, U., & Resnick, M. (1999). Thinking in levels: A dynamic system perspective to making sense of the world. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 8(1), 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilensky, U., & Reisman, K. (2006). Thinking like a wolf, a sheep, or a firefly: Learning biology through constructing and testing computational theories – an embodied modeling approach. Cognition & Instruction, 24(2), 171–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, H.-K. (2010). Modeling a complex system: Using novice-experts analysis for developing an effective technology-enhanced learning environment. International Journal of science Education, 32(2), 195–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yarden, H., & Yarden, A. (2011). Studying biotechnological methods using animations: The teacher’s role. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 20, 689–702.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Billie Eilam .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Eilam, B., Gilbert, J.K. (2014). The Significance of Visual Representations in the Teaching of Science. In: Eilam, B., Gilbert, J. (eds) Science Teachers’ Use of Visual Representations. Models and Modeling in Science Education, vol 8. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-06526-7_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics