Skip to main content

Argument Structure in Peruvian Sign Language

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Formal Approaches to Languages of South America

Abstract

This chapter offers an analysis of Peruvian Sign Language argument structure. It argues, following insights from research on other sign languages, that there is a correlation between the type of certain classifiers and the type of predicates predicted by the Unaccusative Hypothesis. In addition, it shows how the operation Agree values the specific features of each classifier, accounting for their handshape. Furthermore, it provides evidence in favor of the assumption that externalization of sign language syntactic structures follows a layering system, where various pieces of grammatical information can be externalized simultaneously, expressing predicates and arguments in a single sign.

I’d like to express my deep thanks the several Deaf consultants that made research on LSP possible, and also to Alexandra Arnaiz, LSP interpreter, for her invaluable help. Many thanks, too, to Cilene Rodrigues and Andrés Saab for several comments that greatly improved the shape of this chapter. Any remaining flaw is my own.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    It is interesting to notice how effortlessly a prescriptive discourse arises in a linguistic community. Of course, this is not specific of Deaf communities, as can be easily confirmed by the myriad of prescriptivist discussions oral language speakers have all over the world.

  2. 2.

    For the purpose of this chapter, a “native signer” is a LSP signer who has acquired her language from the beginning of her life, i.e., a Deaf individual who has been raised in a Deaf family. This is crucial because, given that Spanish is a SVO language, LSP interpreters (all of them Spanish speakers) tend to sign in SVO fashion, and their LSP (which appears on some TV news programs) is the one with the wider reach among the Deaf community (with occasional complaints about how some specific interpreters sign, however). In fact, interpreters’ representatives are very often asked by government officials to discuss policies regarding the Deaf community’s language. Although Deaf representatives are also invited to those meetings, their opinions have, in the best-case scenario, the same weight as that of interpreters, and quite often they are previously vetted by interpreters. This happens because the Deaf community is not sufficiently organized, and their leaders do not necessarily have the social capital needed to put forward a strong representation. How this affects LSP structure is yet to be studied. With respect to word order, our data points toward SOV, without necessarily precluding alternative analyses. Judy Shepard-Kegl (p.c.) suggested, for instance, that LSP could have a Ground-Figure order.

  3. 3.

    Sentence (1) was offered by the Deaf consultant after being asked (in LSP) to produce an example to illustrate the usage of the verb SEARCH.

  4. 4.

    The issue of simultaneity is not restricted to the externalization of syntactic constituents. It pervades sign language phonology as well. For instance, in (2) the sign MY has different phonological features simultaneously appearing: the handshape (all fingers selected) and the location and movement (directed toward the signer’s chest)—if the same handshape were directed toward the addressee, it would mean YOUR. For a recent discussion about the phonological complexities involved in simultaneous signs, see Sandler (2017).

  5. 5.

    This means we can understand the upper layer as a secondary predicate, something like (5). Or maybe it is a way to introduce a conventional implicature (in the sense of Potts (2005)), as suggested by Andrés Saab (p.c).

  6. 6.

    It is well known that traditional functional notions do not properly capture the complexities involved in the construction of the clause, and many researchers consider them an epiphenomenon (see McCloskey (1997) for a thorough discussion). This has not prevented them from using these expressions (subject, object, etc.) in an informal way, a practice that I follow here.

  7. 7.

    Madrid (2018), following Zwitserlood (2003, 2008, 2012), divides body part classifiers into two groups, assigning them either to handling classifiers or to whole entity classifiers. We are keeping body part classifiers together as a group.

  8. 8.

    An interesting issue is the handshape of the non-dominant hand. It has a pointing finger (but not a classifier). In this case, it is not pointing to the bottle; actually, it is not pointing to anything. I hypothesize that it expresses the stage-level nature of the predicate (pumping); if this is correct, the non-dominant hand is exteriorizing a different proposition, and the pointing finger would be a copula-type expression—see Rodríguez-Mondoñedo and Arnaiz (2022) for the suggestion that LSP may have, or it is developing, a copula from the pronominal form, in line with similar typological evolutions in several languages.

  9. 9.

    Notice that I am not counting the head as a classifier in this case; rather, it is part of another complex system of role shifting, constructed action, and meaningful use of the signing space. We will not discuss this issue here (see Perniss (2012), Lillo-Martin (2012), and references therein).

  10. 10.

    Thus, this is not so different from oral language classifiers, although see below.

  11. 11.

    We will not discuss other types of classifiers—see Supalla (1986), Emmorey (2003), Zwitserlood (2003, 2008, 2012), Sandler and Lillo-Martin (2006), and Madrid (2018), i.a., for further types and careful discussion.

  12. 12.

    Notice that in (15) we are using IX to represent a pointing sign; in this case, IX1 will be the signer pointing to herself to say “I.”

  13. 13.

    Remember we are ignoring null heads, for instance, the T head. There is no evidence that LSP has a morphological expression of Tense, which of course does not preclude a null T. To avoid any commitment, I have opted for labelless trees.

  14. 14.

    Notice that the picture in (22) is a 2D rendition of a 3D handshape and movement. Furthermore, the handshape can be expressed in terms of its phonological features (selected fingers, closure, etc.; see Brentari (2019) for a recent overview of sign language phonology). For LSP, see Raico (in preparation). With respect to the classifier, here we are only dealing with the valuation of its features, not its interpretation status; see Pesetsky and Torrego (2007) for the claim that interpretability and valuation are different aspects of the features.

  15. 15.

    The caveat from fn. 14 also applies here.

  16. 16.

    If we were to implement (30) in terms of Distributed Morphology, we would say that LEG just has an index, and that Agree makes possible the sharing of its index with the classifier. Later, that index will be interpreted as “with legs.”

  17. 17.

    Kimmelman (2022) shows cases where the relation between event structure and classifier type in at least some sign languages is not so straightforward. More research is needed, of course, but the point here is that argument structure does not necessarily predict event structure.

References

  • Abner, Natasha, Kensy Cooperrider, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2015. Gesture for linguists: A handy primer. Language and Linguistics Compass 9 (11): 437–451.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. 2019. Noun categorization devices a cross-linguistic perspective. In Genders and classifiers: A cross-linguistic typology, ed. Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald and Elena I. Mihas, 1–29. Oxford: OUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnaiz, Alexandra. 2021. La lengua de señas peruana como vehículo de aprendizaje del castellano escrito como segunda lengua. MA thesis. Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú. Lima: Perú.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benedicto, Elena, and D. Brentari. 2004. Where did all the arguments go? Argument-changing properties of classifiers in ASL. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 22: 743–810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benedicto, Elena, S. Cvejanov, and J. Quer. 2007. Valency in classifier predicates: A syntactic Analysis. Lingua 117: 1202–1215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brentari, Diane. 2019. Sign language phonology. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Catalán, Yedirel. 2021. Características morfológicas y morfosintácticas en el español escrito por escolares signantes de la lengua de señas peruana (LSP). Tesis de Licenciatura. Lima: UNMSA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cerna-Herrera, Francisco, y César Ramos. 2022. Cláusulas relativas en lengua de señas peruana (LSP). Quintú Quimün. Revista De lingüística 6 (Q063): 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Step by step, ed. R. Martin et al., 89–155. Cambridge, MA: MITPress.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, Brenda. 2017a. Sign language varieties in Lima, Peru. Sign Language Studies 17 (2): 222–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2017b. A grammatical sketch of Sivia sign language. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Hawai`i at Mānoa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuti, Zanabria Elizabeth. 2018. Sistema Antroponímico en la Lengua de Señas Peruana. Tesis de Licenciatura. Lima: UNMSM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, Karen, ed. 2003. Perspectives on classifier constructions in sign languages. Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, Jerry A. 1970. Three reasons for not deriving “kill” from “cause to die”. Linguistic Inquiry 1 (4): 429–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallego, Ángel. 2020. Morpho-syntactic Variation in Romance v: A Micro-parametric Approach. Probus 32 (2): 401–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geraci, Carlo, and Josep Quer. 2014. Determining argument structure in sign languages. In Structuring the Argument: Multidisciplinary research on verb argument structure, ed. Asaf Bachrach, Isabelle Roy, and Linnaea Stockall, 45–60. John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goico, Sara. 2019. The impact of ‘inclusive’ education on the language of deaf youth in Iquitos, Peru. Sign Language Studies 19 (3): 348–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hale, Kenneth, and Samuel J. Keyser. 1993. On Argument Structure and the Lexical Expression of Syntactic Relations. In The view from building 20: Essays in linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger, ed. K. Hale and J. Keyser, 53–109. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hale, Kenneth, and Samuel Jay Keyser. 2005. Aspect and the syntax of argument structure. The syntax of aspect: Deriving thematic and aspectual interpretation. In The syntax of aspect: Deriving thematic and aspectual interpretation, ed. Nomi Erteschik-Shir, Tova R. Rapoport, and Tova Rapoport, 11–41. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • INEI. 2012. Primera Encuesta Nacional Especializada sobre Discapacidad ENEDIS 2012. https://conadisperu.gob.pe/observatorio/estadisticas/encuesta-nacional-especializada-sobre-discapacidad/. Accessed 7 Apr 2022.

  • ———. 2017. CPV 2017 Censo de Población y Vivienda 2017. Lima: Perú. http://censo2017.inei.gob.pe/. Accessed 7 Apr 2022.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, Ray. 1990. Semantic structures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, Richard. 1994. The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimmelman, Vadim. 2022. Argument structure in sign languages. Annual Review of Linguistics 8: 19–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kremers, Joost. 2012. The syntax of simultaneity. Lingua 122 (9): 979–1003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lillo-Martin, Diane. 2012. Utterance reports and constructed action. In Sign language. An international handbook, ed. Roland Pfau, Markus Steinbach, and Bencie Woll, 365–387. De Gruyter Mouton.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lourenço, G., and R.B. Wilbur. 2018. Are plain verbs really plain? Co-localization as the agreement marker in sign languages. FEAST 2: 68–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madrid, Vega Rodrigo. 2018. Clasificadores en la Lengua de Señas Peruana (LSP). Tesis de Licenciatura. Lima: PUCP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malca, Marco, and Frank Domínguez Chenguayen. 2022. Conceptual metonymy in the creation of concrete nominal signs in Peruvian sign language: Towards a metonymic typology. In Concepts, discourses, and translations. Second language learning and teaching, ed. B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk and M. Trojszczak, 81–112. Cham: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, James. 1997. Subjecthood and subject positions. In Elements of grammar: Handbook in generative syntax, ed. Liliane Haegeman, 197–235. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Meir, Irit. 2001. Verb classifiers as noun incorporation in Israeli sign language. In Yearbook of morphology 1999, ed. G. Booij and J. Van Marle, 299–319. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Meir, Irit, Carol Padden, Mark Aronoff, and Wendy Sandler. 2007. Body as subject. Journal of Linguistics. 43: 531–563.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MINEDU. 1987. Lenguaje de Señas Peruanas. Lima: Ministerio de Educación.

    Google Scholar 

  • Napoli, D.J., and R. Sutton-Spence. 2010. Limitations on simultaneity in sign language. Language 86: 647–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olivero, Hugo. 2016. Los sordos peruanos y su lengua ignorada. Lucidez. https://lucidez.pe/los-sordos-peruanos-y-su-lengua-ignorada-por-hugo-olivero/. Accessed 7 Apr 2022.

  • Paliza, Farfan Alberto. 1994. The problem of the Peruvian deaf person. In The Deaf Way: Perspectives from the International Conference on Deaf Culture, ed. C.J. Erting, R.C. Johnson, D.L. Smith, and B.D. Snider, 804–810. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parks, Elizabeth, and Jason Parks. 2009. Sociolinguistic survey report of the deaf community of Peru. Summer Institute of Linguistics Electronic Survey Report 019.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2010. A sociolinguistic profile of the Peruvian deaf community. Sign Language Studies 10 (4): 409–441.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pérez Silva, Jorge. 2021. Forma y sustancia en las señas del lenguaje: un análisis del plano de la expresión de la lengua de señas peruana. In Actualidad y futuro del pensamiento de Eugenio Coseriu. Estudios de teoría del lenguaje, descripción lingüística, dimensión textual y lingüística peruana, ed. Carlos Garatea, Jorge Wiesse, and Marta Fernández Alcaide, 288–304. Madrid: Universidad de Sevilla.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perlmutter, David M. 1978. Impersonal passives and the unaccusative hypothesis. In Proceedings of the 4th annual meeting of the Berkeley linguistics society, 157–189. UC Berkeley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perniss, Pamela. 2012. Use of sign space. In Sign language. An international handbook, ed. Roland Pfau, Markus Steinbach, and Bencie Woll, 412–431. De Gruyter Mouton.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pesetsky, D., and E. Torrego. 2007. The syntax of valuation and the interpretability of features. In Phrasal and clausal architecture, ed. S. Karimi, V. Samiian, and W. Wilkins, 262–294. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pfau, Roland, Martin Salzmann, and Markus Steinbach. 2018. The syntax of sign language agreement: Common ingredients, but unusual recipe. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 3 (1): 1–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potts, Christopher. 2005. The logic of conventional implicatures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylkkänen, Liina. 2002. Introducing arguments. Doctoral dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramchand, G.C. 2008. Verb meaning and the lexicon. A first phase syntax. Cambridge: CUP.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ramos, César. 2022. Los verbos de concordancia doble de la lengua de señas peruana (LSP): un análisis del uso del espacio. Tesis de Licenciatura. Pontificia Universidad Católica del Perú. Lima: Perú.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez-Mondoñedo, Miguel. 1999. La formación de palabras en aimara tupino. Boletín de la Academia Peruana de la Lengua 31: 131–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2020. Privación lingüística: el caso de la lengua de señas. AIEDI https://www.aiedi.org/2020/01/17/privacion-linguistica-el-caso-de-la-lengua-de-senas/. Accessed 7 Apr 2022.

  • ———. 2021. Modificación y desarrollo en lenguas orales y de señas. In Actualidad y futuro del pensamiento de Eugenio Coseriu. Estudios de teoría del lenguaje, descripción lingüística, dimensión textual y lingüística peruana, ed. Carlos Garatea, Jorge Wiesse, and Marta Fernández Alcaide, 305–323. Madrid: Universidad de Sevilla.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez-Mondoñedo, Miguel, and Alexandra Arnaiz. 2022. A copula in Peruvian Sign Language. En semantics of under-represented languages of the Americas (SULA 11), 161–170. GLSA, Amherst: University of Massachusetts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez-Mondoñedo, Miguel, Sonia Maruenda, and Alexandra Arnaiz (Compiladores). 2015. Archivo Digital de la Lengua de Señas Peruana. https://repositorio.pucp.edu.pe/index/handle/123456789/124183. Accessed 7 Apr 2022.

  • Sandler, Wendy. 2017. The challenge of sign language phonology. Annual Review of Linguistics 3: 15.1-15.21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandler, Wendy, and Diane Lillo-Martin. 2006. Sign language and linguistic universals. Cambridge: CUP.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schembri, Adam. 2003. Rethinking “classifiers” in signed languages. In Perspectives on classifier constructions in sign languages, ed. Emmorey Karen, 3–34. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker, Philippe. 2017. Sign language and the foundations of anaphora. Annual Review of Linguistics 3: 149–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Supalla, Ted. 1982. Structure and acquisition of verbs of motion and location in American sign language, PhD thesis, UCSD.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 1986. The classifier system in American sign language. In Noun classes and categorization, ed. C. Craig, 181–214. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Svenonius, Peter. 2016. Spans and words. In Morphological metatheory, ed. Daniel Siddiqi and Heidi Harley, 201–224. John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2020. A span is a thing: A span-based theory of words. NELS 50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, Leonard. 1985. Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Language typology and syntactic description III: Grammatical categories and the lexicon, ed. Timothy Shopen, 57–149. Cambridge: CUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2007. Lexical typologies. In Language typology and syntactic description, ed. Timothy Shopen, vol. III, 66–168. Cambridge: CUP.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Vermeerbergen, L. Leeson, and O. Crasborn, eds. 2007. Simultaneity in sign language: Form and function. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, Alexander. 2015. Arguments in syntax and semantics. Cambridge: CUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilbur, Ronnie. 2003. Modality and the structure of language: Sign languages versus signed systems. In Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education, ed. Marc Marschark and Patricia E. Spencer, 332–346. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwitserlood, Ingeborg. 2003. Classifying hand configurations in sign language of the Netherlands. Doctoral dissertation, Utrecht University.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2008. Morphology below the level of the sign. Frozen forms and classifier predicates. In Signs of the time: Selected papers from TISLR 2004, ed. Josep Quer, 251–272. Hamburg: Signum.

    Google Scholar 

  • ———. 2012. Classifiers. In Sign language. An international handbook, ed. Roland Pfau, Markus Steinbach, and Bencie Woll, 158–185. De Gruyter Mouton.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Miguel Rodríguez-Mondoñedo .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2023 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Rodríguez-Mondoñedo, M. (2023). Argument Structure in Peruvian Sign Language. In: Rodrigues, C., Saab, A. (eds) Formal Approaches to Languages of South America. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-22344-0_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics