Skip to main content

Adding Subjects on the Left

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Developing Language and Literacy

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 23))

  • 444 Accesses

Abstract

Children acquiring English and French do not initially produce any subjects with the verbs that appear in their early two-word utterances. Instead, in both languages children depend heavily on the communicative context for interpretation of what they are trying to say. But they appear to realise early on that subjects are required in these languages. They begin to produce occasional fillers, usually a schwa vowel, in the preverbal slot, and then progress to pronouns and a few lexical nouns in this slot as they produce longer utterances. In this chapter I track the progress children make as they master the options available in English and in French, and show how the forms in adult speech offer somewhat different models in the two languages for how to express subjects. I suggest that the patterns of omission and later use are in large part an effect of information structure, determined by what is given versus new in the current exchange.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Some subjectless forms also appear in writing, in what has been called ‘diary’ style, e.g., Spent the day in the barn, or Have been thinking about last summer (see Schmerling, 1973; Thrasher, 1977).

  2. 2.

    Researchers in the past have counted noncanonical question forms in children as errors (e.g., Brown, 1973, p. 180; Nakayama, 1987, p. 124).

  3. 3.

    None of the parents whose speech Estigarribia (2010) analyzed reached a 90% criterion on canonical question use, the criterion often used to assess children’s acquisition of specific syntactic forms (Brown, 1973).

  4. 4.

    Their ability to combine two words in such utterances depends in part on motor ability as they become more fluent in production (Clark, 1993) and probably in part on the ability to retrieve the words on time from memory.

  5. 5.

    Such entities are also likely to be identified with gaze or gesture as being in joint attention, within an exchange.

  6. 6.

    See, for example, Allen and Schröder 2003 (Inuktitut); Narasimhan et al. 2005 (Hindi); Serratrice 2005 (Italian); Guerriero et al. 2006 (Japanese); Clancy 2003 (Korean); Huang 2011 (Mandarin), and Gürcanli et al. 2007 (Turkish).

  7. 7.

    In French class 1 verbs (ending in -er), these two forms, infinitival (INF) and participial (PP), are homophonous.

  8. 8.

    The combination of lexical nouns and clitic pronouns as subjects has led some researchers to consider clitic pronouns as inflectional elements (e.g., Jakubowicz et al., 1997; Auger, 1995) but others have advanced compelling theoretical and empirical arguments against such an account (see, e.g., Côté, 2001; De Cat, 2005).

  9. 9.

    See also Bottari et al. 1993/1994 (Italian); and Christofidou and Kappa 1998 (Greek).

  10. 10.

    Most of the examples cited come from children between 1;6 and 3;0.

References

  • Allen, S. E. M., & Schröder, H. (2003). Preferred argument structure in early Inuktitut spontaneous speech data. In J. W. DuBois, L. E. Kumpf, & W. J. Ashby (Eds.), Preferred argument structure: Grammar as architecture for function (pp. 301–338). John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, J. E., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2011). Disfluency effects in comprehension: How new information can become accessible. In E. Gibson & N. J. Pearlmutter (Eds.), The processing and acquisition of reference (pp. 197–217). MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, W. J. (1988). The syntax, pragmatics, and sociolinguistics of left- and right-dislocations in French. Lingua, 75, 203–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashby, W. J., & Bentivoglio, P. (1993). Preferred argument structure in spoken French and Spanish. Language Variation and Change, 5, 61–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, M. (1979). Prerequisites for reference. In E. Ochs & B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental pragmatics (pp. 229–249). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Auger, J. (1995). Les clitiques pronominaux en français parlé informel: une approche morphologique. Revue Québécoise de Linguistique, 24, 21–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, N., & Greenfield, P. M. (1988). The development of new and old information young children’s early language. Language Sciences, 10, 3–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassano, D., Maillochon, I., & Eme, E. (1998). Developmental changes and variability in early lexicon: A study of French children’s naturalistic productions. Journal of Child Language, 21, 649–675.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernal, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Millotte, S., & Christophe, A. (2010). Two-year-olds compute syntactic structure on-line. Developmental Science, 13, 69–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanche-Benveniste, C. (1994). Quelques caractéristiques grammaticales des sujets employés dans le français parlé des conversations. In M. Yaguello (Ed.), Subjecthood and subjectivity. The status of the subject in linguistic theory (pp. 77–108). Ophrys.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (1990). Subjectless sentences in child language. Linguistic Inquiry, 21, 491–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, P. (1993). Grammatical continuity in language development: The case of subjectless sentences. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 721–734.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bottari, P., Cipriani, P., & Chilosi, A. M. (1993/1994). Protosyntactic devices in the acquisition of Italian free morphology. Language Acquisition, 3, 327–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1973a). Early syntactic development: A cross-linguistic study with special reference to Finnish. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1973b). Structural relationships in children’s utterances: Syntactic or semantic? In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and acquisition of language (pp. 197–213). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M. D. S. (1963). The ontogeny of English phrase structure: The first phase. Language, 39, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M. D. S. 1976. Children’s first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41 [Serial No. 164].

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. W. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron-Faulkner, T. (2012). A functional account of verb use in the early stages of English multiword development. Journal of Child Language, 39, 885–897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cameron-Faulkner, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2003). A construction based analysis of child directed speech. Cognitive Science, 27, 843–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cartwright, T. A., & Brent, M. R. (1997). Early acquisition of syntactic categories: A formal model. Cognition, 63, 121–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, W. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In C. N. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 25–55). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charney, R. (1980). Speech roles and the development of personal pronouns. Journal of Child Language, 7, 509–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Z., & Valian, V. (2016). Subject production by monolingual English-speaking children: A corpus study. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chouinard, M. M., & Clark, E. V. (2003). Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language, 30, 637–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christofidou, A., & Kappa, I. (1998). Pre-and proto-morphological fillers in Greek language acquisition. In S. Gillis (Ed.), Studies in the acquisition of number and diminutive marking. Antwerp papers in linguistics (Vol. 95, pp. 193–214).

    Google Scholar 

  • Clancy, P. (2003). Preferred argument structure in Korean. In J. W. DuBois, L. E. Kumpf, & W. J. Ashby (Eds.), Preferred argument structure: Grammar as architecture for function (pp. 81–108). John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V. (1978). From gesture to word: On the natural history of deixis. In J. S. Bruner & A. Garton (Eds.), Human growth and development: Wolfson College Lectures 1976 (pp. 85–120). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V. (1993). The Lexicon in Acquisition. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V. (1998). Lexique et syntaxe dans l’acquisition du français. Langue Française, 118, 49–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V. (2020). Conversational repair and the acquisition of language. Discourse Processes, 57, 441–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V., & de Marneffe, M.-C. (2012). Constructing verb paradigms in French: Adult construals and emerging grammatical contrast. Morphology, 22, 89–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E. V., & Veneziano, E. (2013, October). Constructing subjects: Fillers, clitics, and nominals. Paper presented at the Conference on ‘Acquisition des Expressions Référentielles,’ Université de Paris-Descartes, Paris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Côté, M.-H. (2001). On the status of subject clitics in Child French. In E. Kidd et al. (Eds.), Research on child language acquisition: Proceedings of the 8th conference of the international association for the study of child language (pp. 1314–1330). Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Couper-Kuhlen, E., & Ono, T. (2007). ‘Incrementing’ in conversation: Comparison of practices in English, German, and Japanese. Pragmatics, 17, 513–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Culbertson, J. (2010). Convergent evidence for categorial change in French: From subject clitic to agreement marker. Language, 86, 85–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Cat, C. (2005). French subject clitics are not agreement markers. Lingua, 115, 1195–1219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Cat, C. (2009). Experimental evidence for preschoolers’ mastery of ‘topic’. Language Acquisition, 16, 224–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Pontonx, S., Masson, C., Leroy-Collombel, M., & Morgenstern, A. (2014, July). Ça s’appelle comment le docteur des animaux? Co-(re)construction des productions nonstandard de l’enfant. 4e Congrès Mondial de Linguistique Française, Berlin, Germany. EDP Sciences, 8, 1453–1467. SHS Web of Conferences 8. https://doi.org/10.1051/shsconf/20140801189

  • Deutsch, W., & Pechmann, T. (1978). Ihr, dir, or mir? On the acquisition of pronouns in German children. Cognition, 6, 155–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dye, C. (2011). Reduced auxiliaries in early child language: Converging observational and experimental evidence from French. Journal of Linguistics, 47, 301–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estigarribia, B. (2010). Facilitation by variation: Right-to-left learning of English Yes/No questions. Cognitive Science, 34, 68–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Firbas, J. (1992). Functional sentence perspective in written and spoken communication. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fonseca-Greber, B., & Waugh, L. R. (2003). On the radical difference between the subject personal pronouns in written and spoken European French. In P. Leustyna & C. F. Meyer (Eds.), Corpus analysis: Language structure and language use (pp. 225–240). Brill Rodopi.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geluykens, R. (1987). Tails (right-dislocations) as a repair mechanism in English conversation. In J. Nuyts & G. de Schutter (Eds.), Getting one’s words into Line (pp. 119–130). De Gruyter Mouton.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Geluykens, R. (1993). Topic introduction in English conversation. Transactions of Philological Society, 91, 181–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerken, L.-A. (1991). The metrical basis for children’s subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 431–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Girouard, P. C., Ricard, M., & Décarie, T. G. (1997). The acquisition of personal pronouns in French-speaking and English-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 24, 311–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, E., Theakston, A., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Subject and object omission in children’s early transitive constructions: A discourse-pragmatic approach. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 701–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M., & Smith, J. H. (1976). The structure of communication in early language development. Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grévisse, M., & Goosse, A. (2011). Le Bon usage. Duculot/De Boeck.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, J. S. (1967). Topicalization in child language. Foundations of Language, 3(1), 37–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guerriero, A. M. S., Oshima-Takane, Y., & Kuriyama, Y. (2006). The development of referential choice in English and Japanese: A discourse-pragmatic perspective. Journal of Child Language, 33, 853–857.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gürcanli, Ö., Demiralp, M. N., & Ozyurek, A. (2007). Shared information and argument omission in Turkish. In H. Caunt-Nulton, S. Kulatilake, & I. Woo (Eds.), 31st Annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 267–273). Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean. In E. H. Lenneberg & E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Foundations of language development (pp. 239–265). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamann, C., Rizzi, L., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (1996). On the acquisition of subject and object clitics in French. In H. Clahsen (Ed.), Generative perspectives on language aacquisition (pp. 309–335). John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haviland, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1974). What’s new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 512–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hickmann, M., & Hendriks, H. (1999). Cohesion and anaphora in children’s narratives: A comparison of English, French, German, and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language, 26, 419–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, C. (2011). Referential choice in Mandarin child language: A discourse-pragmatic perspective. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 2057–2080.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, M. E., & Allen, S. E. M. (2006). A discourse-pragmatic analysis of subject omission in child English. In D. Bamman, T. Magnitskaia, & C. Zeller (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University conference on language development (Vol. 1, pp. 293–304). Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, M. E., & Allen, S. E. M. (2013). The effect of individual discourse-pragmatic features on referential choice in child English. Journal of Pragmatics, 56, 15–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huttenlocher, J., Smiley, P., & Charney, R. (1983). Emergence of action categories in the child: Evidence from verb meanings. Psychological Review, 90, 72–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jakubowicz, C., Müller, N., Riemer, B., & Rigaut, C. (1997). The case of subject and object omissions in French and German. In E. Hughes, M. Hughes, & A. Greenhill (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st annual Boston University conference on language development (pp. 331–342). Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jourdain, M. (2020). A quantitative and qualitative corpus study of the acquisition of topic constructions in Child French. PhD dissertation, Université de Lille and KU Leuven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jourdain, M., & Lahousse, K. (2021). The development of constructions from the right edge: A multinominal regression analysis of clitic left and right dislocation in child French. Journal of Child Language, 00(0), 000–000. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000513

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jourdain, M., Canut, E., & Lahousse, K. (2020a). The L1 acquisition of dislocation in French: A usage-based analysis of information structure. In M. M. Brown & A. Kohut (Eds.), Proceedings of the 44th Boston University conference on language development (pp. 238–251). Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jourdain, M., Canut, E., & Lahousse, K. (2020b). Item-based acquisition of dislocation in early child French. Journal of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, 2, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1981). The grammatical marking of thematic structure in the development of language production. In W. Deutsch (Ed.), The child’s construction of language (pp. 121–147). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kedar, Y., Casasola, M., Lust, B., & Parmet, Y. (2017). Little words, big impact: Determiners begin to bootstrap reference by 12 months. Language Learning and Development, 13, 317–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, E. O., & Schieffelin, B. B. (1976). Topic as a discourse notion: A study of topic in the conversations of children and adults. In C. N. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp. 335–384). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambrecht, K. (1981). Topic, antitopic and verb agreement in non-standard French. John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambrecht, K. (1994). Information structure and sentence form: Topic, focus, and the mental representations of discourse referents. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Legendre, G., Culbertson, J., Barrière, I., & Goyet, L. (2010). Experimental and empirical evidence for the status and acquisition of subject clitics and agreement marking in adult and child Spoken French. In V. Torrrens, L. Escobar, A. Gavarro, & J. Gutierrez (Eds.), Movement and clitics: Adult and child grammar (pp. 333–360). Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M., & Feider, H. (1986). The acquisition of person pronouns in French-speaking children. International Journal of Psychology, 21, 629–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leroy-Collombel, M. (2009). La reformulation dans les interactions adulte-enfant: une analyse longitudinale de 1;06 à 2;08 ans. Cahiers de Praxématique, 52, 59–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieven, E., Pine, J. M., & Baldwin, G. (1997). Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language, 24, 187–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcos, H., Salazar Orvig, A., da Silva-Genest, C., & Heurdier, J. (2021). The influence of dialogue in young children’s uses of referring expressions. In A. Salazar Orvig, G. de Weck, R. Hassan, & A. Rialland (Eds.), The acquisition of referring expressions: A dialogical approach (pp. 000–000). John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, D., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2010). What’s in a manner of speaking? Children’s sensitivity to partner-specific referential precedents. Developmental Psychology, 46, 749–760.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, D., Butcher, J., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Two- and four-year-olds learn to adapt referring expressions in context: Effects of distracters and feedback on referential communications. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4, 184–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mintz, T. H. (2003). Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech. Cognition, 90, 91–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgenstern, A., & Parisse, C. (2012). The Paris corpus. Journal of French Language Studies, 22, 7–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakayama, M. (1987). Performance factors in subject-auxiliary inversion by children. Journal of Child Language, 14, 113–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narasimhan, B., & Dimroth, C. (2008). Word order and information status in child language. Cognition, 107, 317–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narasimhan, B., Budwig, N., & Murty, L. (2005). Argument realization in Hindi caregiver-child discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 461–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. E. (1977). Facilitating children’s syntax acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 13, 101–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ninio, A. (1992). The relation of children’s single word utterances to single word utterances in the input. Journal of Child Language, 19, 87–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Grady, W., Peters, A. M., & Masterson, D. (1989). The transition from optional to required subjects. Journal of Child Language, 16, 513–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pepinsky, T., Demuth, K., & Roark, B. (2001). The status of filler syllables in children’s early speech. In A. H.-J. Do et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 25th Boston University conference on child language development (pp. 575–586). Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, A. M., & Menn, L. (1993). False starts and filler syllables: Ways to learn grammatical morphemes. Language, 69, 742–777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. (1990). The who, when, and where of early narratives. Journal of Child Language, 17, 433–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pine, J., & Lieven, E. (1997). Slot and frame patterns in the development of the determiner category. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 123–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., & Svartvik, J. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of the English language. Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozendaal, M., & Baker, A. (2010). The acquisition of reference: Pragmatic aspects and the influence of language input. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 1866–1879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salazar Orvig, A., Marcos, H., Morgenstern, A., Hassan, R., Leber-Marin, J., & Parès, J. (2010). Dialogical factors in toddlers’ use of clitic pronouns. First Language, 30, 375–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmerling, S. (1973). Subjectless sentences and the notion of surface structure. In C. Corum, T. C. Smith-Stark, & A. Weiner (Eds.), Proceedings of the ninth regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society (pp. 577–586). Chicago Linguistics Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, K. (2007). L’interface syntaxe-pragmatique: Le sujet chez des enfants bilingues franco-allemands et italo-allemands. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Etrangère, 25, 9–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebastian-Gallés, N. (2007). Biased to learn language. Developmental Science, 10, 713–718.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Serratrice, L. (2005). The role of discourse pragmatics in the acquisition of subjects in Italian. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 437–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shi, R., & Melançon, A. (2010). Syntactic categorization in French-learning infants. Infancy, 15(5), 517–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thrasher, R. (1977). One way to say more by saying less: A study of so-called subjectless sentences. Kwansei Gakuin University Monograph Series, 11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Umiker-Sebeok, D. J. (1979). Preschool children’s intraconversational narratives. Journal of Child Language, 6, 91–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valian, V. (1991). Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition, 40, 21–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valian, V., Chodorow, M., & Chen, Z. C. (2020). Subjects and inflections pattern similarly in English-speaking two-year-olds and adults. Unpublished manuscript, Hunter College, NYC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veneziano, E., & Clark, E. V. (2016). Early verb constructions in French: Adjacency on the left edge. Journal of Child Language, 43, 1193–1230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veneziano, E., & Sinclair, H. (2000). The changing status of ‘filler syllables’ on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language, 27, 461–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Virbel, M. (1975). Etude du Détachement et de l’Extraction dans le Langage de l’Enfant. Thèse du Doctorat de Troisième Cycle, Université de Provence, Centre d’Aix-Marseille.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisleder, A., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). What’s in the input? Frequent frames in child-directed speech offer distributional cues to grammatical categories in Spanish and English. Journal of Child Language, 37, 1089–1108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eve V. Clark .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Clark, E.V. (2022). Adding Subjects on the Left. In: Levie, R., Bar-On, A., Ashkenazi, O., Dattner, E., Brandes, G. (eds) Developing Language and Literacy. Literacy Studies, vol 23. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-99891-2_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-99891-2_2

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-99890-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-99891-2

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics