Skip to main content

Problem Finding

  • Reference work entry
  • First Online:
The Palgrave Encyclopedia of the Possible
  • 36 Accesses

Abstract

Problem finding or problem construction is commonly seen as the first step in creative problem-solving. Problem construction consists of two main aspects. The first aspect focuses on the identification of problems in the environment. The second aspect of problem construction focuses on the structure and description of a problem in order to then create a solution. The problem construction process most often occurs quickly and automatically; however, actively engaging in problem construction has been linked to an increase in creativity. Additionally, problem construction has been associated with creativity across a wide range of domains. There are a variety of factors that may affect problem construction, such as expertise and personality variables, as well as the amount of time or information provided to the problem-solver. While problem construction tends to be automatic, training in problem construction or instructions to actively engage in problem construction increase creativity. Engaging in the problem construction process takes time and requires effort on the part of the problem-solver. Problem construction provides a road map for the possible. Goals identified in the problem construction process relate to what is possible, which constraints relate to what is impossible.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 849.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 899.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abdulla, A. M., Paek, S. H., Cramond, B., & Runco, M. A. (2018). Problem finding and creativity: A meta-analytic review. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000194.

  • Baer, J. M. (1988). Long-term effects of creativity training with middle school students. Journal of Early Adolescence, 8, 183–193. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431688082006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basadur, M. (1995). Optimal ideation-evaluation ratios. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 63–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. B., Scherer, L. L., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2003). Effects of solution elicitation aids and need for cognition on the generation of solutions to ill-structured problems. Creativity Research Journal, 15(2), 235–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Getzels, J. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). From problem-solving to problem finding, In I. A. Taylor & J. W. Getzels (Eds.), Perspectives in Creativity (pp. 90–116). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. (1967). Creativity: Yesterday, today, and tomorrow. Journal of Creative Behavior, 1, 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holyoak, K. (1984). Mental models in problem solving. In J. R. Anderson & K. M. Kosslyn (Eds.), Tutorials in working memory (pp. 193–218). New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ma, H. H. (2009). The effect size of variables associated with creativity: A meta-analysis. Creativity Research Journal, 21(1), 30–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400410802633400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mumford, M. D., Mobley, M. I., Reiter-Palmon, R., Uhlman, C. E., & Doares, L. M. (1991). Process analytic models of creative capacities. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 91–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mumford, M. D., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Redmond, M. R. (1994). Problem construction and cognition: Applying problem representations in ill-defined domains. In M. A. Runco (Ed.), Problem finding, problem solving, and creativity (pp. 3–39). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Okuda, S. M., Runco, M. A., & Berger, D. E. (1991). Creativity and the finding and solving of real-world problems. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 9, 45–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redmond, M., Mumford, M., & Teach, R. (1993). Putting creativity to work: Effects of leader behavior on subordinate creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 55, 120–151. https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1993.1027.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiter-Palmon, R., & Robinson, E. (2009). Problem identification and construction: What do we know, what is the future? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 43–47. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiter-Palmon, R., Mumford, M., O’Connor Boes, J., & Runco, M. (1997). Problem construction and creativity: The role of ability, cue consistency and active processing. Creativity Research Journal, 10, 9–23. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1001_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiter-Palmon, R., Mumford, M. D., & Threlfall, K. V. (1998). Solving everyday problems creatively: The role of problem construction and personality type. Creativity Research Journal, 11(3), 187–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rostan, S. M. (1997). A study of young artists: The development of artistic talent and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 10(2–3), 175–192. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1002&3_6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M., & Okuda, S. (1988). Problem discovery, divergent thinking, and the creative process. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 211–220. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01538162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, G., Leritz, L. E., & Mumford, M. D. (2004). The effectiveness of creativity training: A quantitative review. Creativity Research Journal, 16(4), 361–388. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400410409534549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, P. D. (2014). Thinking inside the tool box: Creativity, constraints, and the colossal portraits of chuck close. Journal of Creative Behavior, 48, 276–289. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vernon, D., & Hocking, I. (2016). Beyond belief: Structured techniques prove more effective than a placebo intervention in a problem construction task. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 19, 153–159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2015.10.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Roni Reiter-Palmon .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Section Editor information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this entry

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this entry

Reiter-Palmon, R., Ahmed, S. (2022). Problem Finding. In: Glăveanu, V.P. (eds) The Palgrave Encyclopedia of the Possible. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-90913-0_72

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics