Skip to main content

The Influence of Low- and Mid-Level Visual Features on the Perception of Streetscape Qualities

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Human Perception of Visual Information

Abstract

Recent research has quantified aspects of the built environment to understand which visual features may play a role in environmental preferences and behavior. The occurrence of different low-level (e.g., spatial and color properties) and mid-level features (e.g., objects) that mimic patterns seen in nature may influence the quality of pedestrian experiences in urban environments. For example, buildings harboring low-level visual features/patterns that mimic that of nature were perceived to be more naturalistic and significantly predicted preference ratings for architectural scenes. In addition, mid-level visual features with natural elements (e.g., green plants, trees) existing in urban environments increased positive pedestrian experiences, such as feelings of safety, liveliness and beauty. By quantifying visual characteristics and design qualities, one can relate more abstract aspects of an urban space to quantifiable design features, which provides the opportunity to establish a causal relationship between design features and psychological feelings such as walkability, preference, visual complexity and disorder. This research combining perception, computer vision and urban design could help to create more preferable and walkable urban centers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Appleton, J. (1996). The experience of landscape. Wiley Chichester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bar, M., & Neta, M. (2006). Humans prefer curved visual objects. Psychological science, 17(8), 645–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barton, J., & Pretty, J. (2010). What is the best dose of nature and green exercise for improving mental health? a multi-study analysis. Environmental science & technology, 44(10), 3947–3955.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, M. G., Jonides, J., & Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychological Science, 19(12), 1207–1212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, M. G., Kross, E., Krpan, K. M., Askren, M. K., Burson, A., Deldin, P. J., …Jonides, J. (2012). Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 140(3), 300–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, M. G., Hout, M. C., Kardan, O., Hunter, M. R., Yourganov, G., Henderson, J. M., …Jonides, J. (2014). The perception of naturalness correlates with low-level visual features of environmental scenes. PloS one, 9(12), e114572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, M. G., Kardan, O., Kotabe, H. P., Nusbaum, H. C., & London, S. E. (2019a). The promise of environmental neuroscience. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 414–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, M. G., Stier, A. J., & Akcelik, G. N. (2019b). Environmental neuroscience. American Psychologist, 74(9), 1039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertamini, M., Palumbo, L., Gheorghes, T. N., & Galatsidas, M. (2016). Do observers like curvature or do they dislike angularity? British Journal of Psychology, 107(1), 154–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berto, R. (2005). Exposure to restorative environments helps restore attentional capacity. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25(3), 249–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, D. E., Buyung-Ali, L. M., Knight, T. M., & Pullin, A. S. (2010). A systematic review of evidence for the added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health, 10(1), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bratman, G. N., Daily, G. C., Levy, B. J., & Gross, J. J. (2015). The benefits of nature experience: Improved affect and cognition. Landscape and Urban Planning, 138, 41–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camgoz, N., & Yener, C. (2002). Effects of hue, saturation, and brightness on preference: a study on goethe’s color circle with rgb color space. In 9th congress of the international colour association (Vol. 4421, pp. 392–395).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassarino, M., & Setti, A. (2016). Complexity as key to designing cognitive-friendly environments for older people. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chavis, D. M., & Wandersman, A. (2002). Sense of community in the urban environment: A catalyst for participation and community development. In A quarter century of community psychology (pp. 265–292). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coburn, A., Kardan, O., Kotabe, H., Steinberg, J., Hout, M. C., Robbins, A., …Berman, M. G. (2019). Psychological responses to natural patterns in architecture. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 62, 133–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiCarlo, J. J., & Cox, D. D. (2007). Untangling invariant object recognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 333–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, L. E., S”utterlin, S., & Calogiuri, G. (2020). Proposing a framework for the restorative effects of nature through conditioning: Conditioned restoration theory. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(18), 6792.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ewing, R., & Handy, S. (2009). Measuring the unmeasurable:Urban design qualities related to walkability. Journal of Urban Design, 14(1), 65–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ewing, R. H., et al. (1996). Pedestrian-and transit-friendly design.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ewing, R., Handy, S., Brownson, R. C., Clemente, O., & Winston, E. (2006). Identifying and measuring urban design qualities related to walkability. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 3(s1), S223–S240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gladwell, V., Brown, D., Barton, J. L., Tarvainen, M., Kuoppa, P., Pretty, J., …Sandercock, G. (2012). The effects of views of nature on autonomic control. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 112(9), 3379–3386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grahn, P., & Stigsdotter, U. A. (2003). Landscape planning and stress. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 2(1), 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Handy, S. L. (1992). Regional versus local accessibility: neo-traditional development and its implications for non-work travel. Built Environment (1978-), 253–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartig, T., van den Berg, A. E., Hagerhall, C. M., Tomalak, M., Bauer,N., Hansmann, R., …others (2011). Health benefits of nature experience: Psychological, social and cultural processes. In Forests, trees and human health (pp. 127–168). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartig, T., Mitchell, R., De Vries, S., & Frumkin, H. (2014). Nature and health. Annual Review of Public Health, 35, 207–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • He, K., Zhang, X., Ren, S., & Sun, J. (2016). Deep residual learning for image recognition. In Proceedings of the ieee conference on computer vision and pattern recognition (pp. 770–778).

    Google Scholar 

  • He, K., Gkioxari, G., Dollár, P., & Girshick, R. (2017). Mask r-cnn. In Proceedings of the ieee international conference on computer vision (pp. 2961–2969).

    Google Scholar 

  • Herzog, T. R., Maguire, P., Nebel, M. B., et al. (2003). Assessing the restorative components of environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23(2), 159–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, G., Deng, L., Yu, D., Dahl, G. E., Mohamed, A.-r., Jaitly, N., …others (2012). Deep neural networks for acoustic modeling in speech recognition: The shared views of four research groups. IEEE Signal Processing Magazine, 29(6), 82–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, M. R., & Askarinejad, A. (2015). Designer’s approach for scene selection in tests of preference and restoration along a continuum of natural to manmade environments. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ibarra, F. F., Kardan, O., Hunter, M. R., Kotabe, H. P., Meyer, F. A., & Berman, M. G. (2017). Image feature types and their predictions of aesthetic preference and naturalness. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, A. B. (1993). Great streets: Monument avenue, richmond, virginia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joye, Y. (2007). Architectural lessons from environmental psychology: The case of biophilic architecture. Review of General Psychology, 11(4), 305–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joye, Y., & Van den Berg, A. (2011). Is love for green in our genes? a critical analysis of evolutionary assumptions in restorative environments research. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 10(4), 261–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, R., & Kaplan, S. (1989). The experience of nature: A psychological perspective. Cambridge university press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, S., Kaplan, R., & Wendt, J. S. (1972). Rated preference and complexity for natural and urban visual material. Perception & Psychophysics, 12(4), 354–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kardan, O., Demiralp, E., Hout, M. C., Hunter, M. R., Karimi, H., Hanayik, T., …Berman, M. G. (2015). Is the preference of natural versus man-made scenes driven by bottom-up processing of the visual features of nature? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keizer, K., Lindenberg, S., & Steg, L. (2008). The spreading of disorder. Science, 322(5908), 1681–1685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellert, S. R. (2012). Building for life: Designing and understanding the humannature connection. Island press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kjellgren, A., & Buhrkall, H. (2010). A comparison of the restorative effect of a natural environment with that of a simulated natural environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30(4), 464–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotabe, H. P. (2014). The world is random: a cognitive perspective on perceived disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotabe, H. P., Kardan, O., & Berman, M. G. (2016). The order of disorder: Deconstructing visual disorder and its effect on rule-breaking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(12), 1713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotabe, H. P., Kardan, O., & Berman, M. G. (2017). The nature-disorder paradox: A perceptual study on how nature is disorderly yet aesthetically preferred. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(8), 1126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, F. E., & Sullivan, W. C. (2001). Aggression and violence in the inner city: Effects of environment via mental fatigue. Environment and Behavior, 33(4), 543–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lecic-Tosevski, D. (2019). Is urban living good for mental health? Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 32(3), 204–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lederbogen, F., Kirsch, P., Haddad, L., Streit, F., Tost, H., Schuch, P., …others (2011). City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress processing in humans. Nature, 474(7352), 498–501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lo, R. H. (2009). Walkability: what is it? Journal of Urbanism, 2(2), 145–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • McMahan, E. A., & Estes, D. (2015). The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: A meta-analysis. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 10(6), 507–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meidenbauer, K. L., Stenfors, C.U., Bratman, G.N., Gross, J. J., Schertz, K. E., Choe, K. W., & Berman, M. G. (2020). The affective benefits of nature exposure: What’s nature got to do with it? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 72, 101498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merriam-Webster. (n.d). Meme. In Merriam-Webster.com dictionary. Retrieved October 7, 2020, from https://www.merriamwebster.com/dictionary/meme

  • Mitchell, L., & Burton, E. (2006). Neighbourhoods for life: Designing dementiafriendly outdoor environments. Quality in Ageing and Older Adults, 7(1), 26–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, D. T., Farrell, C., & Welsh, B. C. (2019). Broken (windows) theory: A meta-analysis of the evidence for the pathways from neighborhood disorder to resident health outcomes and behaviors. Social Science & Medicine, 228, 272–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohly, H., White, M. P., Wheeler, B.W., Bethel, A., Ukoumunne, O. C., Nikolaou, V., & Garside, R. (2016). Attention restoration theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health, Part B, 19(7), 305–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oliva, A., & Torralba, A. (2006). Building the gist of a scene: The role of global image features in recognition. Progress in Brain Research, 155, 23–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, J. W. (2015). Understanding mid-level representations in visual processing. Journal of Vision, 15(7), 5–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti, T., Lobel, H., Rocco, V., & Hurtubia, R. (2019). Explaining subjective perceptions of public spaces as a function of the built environment: A massive data approach. Landscape and Urban Planning, 181, 169–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schertz, K. E., & Berman, M. G. (2019). Understanding nature and its cognitive benefits. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(5), 496–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schertz, K. E., Sachdeva, S., Kardan, O., Kotabe, H. P., Wolf, K. L., & Berman, M. G. (2018). Athought in the park: The influence of naturalness and low-level visual features on expressed thoughts. Cognition, 174, 82–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schertz, K. E., Saxon, J., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Bettencourt, L., Ding, Y., Hoffmann, H., & Berman, M. (2019). Neighborhood street activity and greenspace usage uniquely contribute to predicting crime. Preprint.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schertz, K. E., Kardan, O., & Berman, M. G. (2020). Visual features influence thought content in the absence of overt semantic information. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82(8), 3945–3956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J., & Barona, C. M. (2009). Do people prefer curved objects? angularity, expertise, and aesthetic preference. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 27(1), 25–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Southworth, M. (2005). Designing the walkable city. Journal of Urban Planning and Development, 131(4), 246–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stenfors, C. U., Van Hedger, S. C., Schertz, K. E., Meyer, F. A., Smith, K. E., Norman, G. J., …others (2019). Positive effects of nature on cognitive performance across multiple experiments: Test order but not affect modulates the cognitive effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, M. P., Schilhab, T., & Bentsen, P. (2018). Attention restoration theory ii: A systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure to natural environments. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health, Part B, 21(4), 227–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sugihara, G., May, R., Ye, H., Hsieh, C.-h., Deyle, E., Fogarty, M., & Munch, S. (2012). Detecting causality in complex ecosystems. Science, 338(6106), 496–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • TripleQ. (2014). Kirby is shaped like a friend. Retrieved 2020-11-08, from https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=aPouHBYs6IY.

  • Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., & Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11(3), 201–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valtchanov, D., Barton, K. R., & Ellard, C. (2010). Restorative effects of virtual nature settings. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13(5), 503–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valtchanov, D., & Ellard, C. G. (2015). Cognitive and affective responses to natural scenes: effects of low level visual properties on preference, cognitive load and eye-movements. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 43, 184–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hedger, S. C., Nusbaum, H. C., Clohisy, L., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., & Berman, M. G. (2019a). Of cricket chirps and car horns: The effect of nature sounds on cognitive performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(2), 522–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hedger, S. C., Nusbaum, H. C., Heald, S. L., Huang, A., Kotabe, H. P., & Berman, M. G. (2019b). The aesthetic preference for nature sounds depends on sound object recognition. Cognitive Science, 43(5), e12734.

    Google Scholar 

  • Velarde, M. D., Fry, G., & Tveit, M. (2007). Health effects of viewing landscapes-landscape types in environmental psychology. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 6(4), 199–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, K. (2013). Causality between built environment and travel behavior: Structural equations model applied to southern california. Transportation Research Record, 2397(1), 80–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, J. Q., & Kelling, G. L. (1982). Broken windows. Atlantic Monthly, 249(3), 29–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xiao, J., Owens, A., & Torralba, A. (2013). Sun3d: A database of big spaces reconstructed using sfm and object labels. In Proceedings of the ieee international conference on computer vision (pp. 1625–1632).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, F., Zhou, B., Liu, L., Liu, Y., Fung, H. H., Lin, H., & Ratti, C. (2018). Measuring human perceptions of a large-scale urban region using machine learning. Landscape and Urban Planning, 180, 148–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhao, H., Shi, J., Qi, X., Wang, X., & Jia, J. (2017). Pyramid scene parsing network. In Proceedings of the ieee conference on computer vision and pattern recognition (pp. 2881–2890).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gaby N. Akcelik .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Akcelik, G.N., Schertz, K.E., Berman, M.G. (2022). The Influence of Low- and Mid-Level Visual Features on the Perception of Streetscape Qualities. In: Ionescu, B., Bainbridge, W.A., Murray, N. (eds) Human Perception of Visual Information. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-81465-6_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-81465-6_9

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-81464-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-81465-6

  • eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics