Skip to main content

Infancy Through the Lens of Evolutionary Developmental Science

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Evolutionary Perspectives on Infancy

Part of the book series: Evolutionary Psychology ((EVOLPSYCH))

  • 732 Accesses

Abstract

Natural selection has operated more strongly on infancy than other stages of development. Infants are born with low-level perceptual and cognitive biases to attend to and process some information more readily than others. Because of infants’ high degree of plasticity, these biases can result in a range of outcomes, depending upon an infant’s specific experiences; however, most infants will develop species-typical behavior assuming exposure to a species-typical environment. Some adaptations of infancy serve as the foundation for later, more sophisticated processes (deferred adaptations), whereas others serve to adapt infants to their current environment and disappear when they are no longer needed (ontogenetic adaptations). Infancy is defined differently by different researchers: (1) the first year of life, reflecting the substantial changes in cognition and behavior over a brief period of time; (2) the first two years of life, corresponding to the acquisition of language and the transition from a mostly sensorimotor to symbolic form of representation; and (3) from birth to weaning (about 3 years), permitting contrasts with nonhuman mammals. The remainder of the chapter summarizes the contributions to the volume.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aknin, L. B., Broesch, T., Hamlin, J. K., & Van de Vondervoort, J. W. (2015). Prosocial behavior leads to happiness in a small-scale rural society. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 788–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anzures, G., Wheeler, A., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., Heron-Delaney, M., Tanaka, J. W., & Lee, K. (2012). Brief daily exposure to Asian females reverses perceptual narrowing for Asian faces in Caucasian infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 484–495.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, P. J. (1993). Memory for gender-consistent and gender-inconsistent event sequences by twenty-five-month-old children. Child Development, 64, 285–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). A note on neonatal imitation. Developmental Review, 7, 86–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role of immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 153–169.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (2015). Developing adaptations. Developmental Review, 38, 13–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (2018). A metatheory for cognitive development (or “Piaget is dead” revisited). Child Development, 89, 2288–2302.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., Ellis, B. J., & Rosenberg, J. S. (2007). Evolved probabilistic cognitive mechanisms. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 35, pp. 1–39). Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B. (2021). Patterns of human growth (3rd ed.). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38, 958–966.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bründl, A. C., Tkaczynski, P. J., Nohon Kohou, G., Boesch, C., Wittig, R. M., & Crockford, C. (2020). Systematic mapping of developmental milestones in wild chimpanzees. Developmental Science, 24(1), e12988.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M. M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-olds. Child Development, 84, 422–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Callaghan, T. C., Moll, H., Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F. L. U., Behne, T., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. Monographs of the Society of Research in Child Development, 76(2) (Serial No. 299).

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1998). 14- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior & Development, 21, 315–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crabtree, J. W., & Riesen, A. H. (1979). Effects of the duration of dark rearing on visually guided behavior in the kitten. Developmental Psychobiology, 12, 291–303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidov, M., Paz, Y., Roth-Hanania, R., Uzefovsky, F., Orlitsky, T., Mankuta, D., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2020). Caring babies: Concern for others in distress during infancy. Developmental Science, e13016.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Beer, G. (1958). Embryos and ancestors (3rd ed.). Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Klerk, C. C. J. M., Bulgarelli, C., & Hamilton, A. (2019). Selective facial mimicry of native over foreign speakers in preverbal infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 183, 33–47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Csbra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 9602–9605.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gave, L., Valenza, E., Turati, C., & De Schonen, S. (2008). Effect of partial occlusion on newborns’ face preference and recognition. Developmental Science, 11, 563–574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50, 24–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Development, 71, 57–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griffey, J. A. F., & Little, A. C. (2014). Infants’ visual preferences for facial traits associated with adult attractiveness judgments: Data from eye-tracking. Infant Behavior & Development, 37, 268–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haith, M. M. (1966). The response of the human newborn to visual movement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 235–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M. (1989). Neonatal imitation gaze aversion and mother-infant interaction. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 495–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937–944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. Journal of Physiology, 160, 106–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, F. J., Liu, S., Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., & Ge, L. (2009). Development of the other-race effect in infancy: Evidence toward universality? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 105–114.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Le Grand, R., Mondloch, C. J., Maurer, D., & Brent, H. P. (2001). Early visual experience and face processing. Nature, 410, 890.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lewkowicz, D. J. (2014). Early experience and multisensory perceptual narrowing. Developmental Psychobiology, 56, 292–315.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 173–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10, F1–F7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Macchi Cassia, V., Turati, C., & Simion, F. (2004). Can a nonspecific bias toward top-heavy patterns explain newborns’ face preference? Psychological Science, 15(6), 379–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marchetto, M. C., Hrvoj-Mihic, B., Kerman, B. E., Yu, D. X., Vadodaria, K. C., Linker, S. B., Narvaiza, I., Santos, R., Denli, A. M., Mendes, A. P., Oefner, R., Cook, J., McHenry, L., Grasmick, J. M., Heard, K., Fredlender, C., Randolph-Moore, L., Kshirsagar, R., Xenitopoulos, R., … Gage, F. H. (2019). Species-specific maturation profiles of human, chimpanzee and bonobo neural cells. ELife, 8, e37527.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybala, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903–933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, D., & Lewis, T. L. (2013). Sensitive periods in visual development. In P. D. Zelazo (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 202–234). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, D., Mondloch, C. J., & Lewis, T. L. (2007). Effects of early visual deprivation on perceptual and cognitive development. Progress in Brain Research, 164, 87–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. L., Budreau, D. R., Maurer, D., Dannemiller, J. L., Stephens, B. R., & Kleiner-Gathercoal, K. A. (1999). Face perception during early infancy. Psychological Science, 10, 419–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Davis, J., Clark, S., & Slaughter, V. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26, 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheim, R. W. (1981). Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive processes in the development of the nervous system and behavior. In K. J. Connolly & H. F. R. Prechtl (Eds.), Maturation and development: Biological and psychological perspectives (pp. 73–108). International Medical Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life? Science, 296, 1321–1323.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pascalis, O., Scott, L. S., Kelly, D. J., Shannon, R. W., Nicholson, E., Coleman, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2005). Plasticity of face processing in infancy. PNAS, 102, 5297–5300.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Periss, V., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2011). Trials and tribulations of childhood: An evolutionary perspective. In C. Salmon & T. Shackelford (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary family psychology (pp. 149–168). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Portmann, A. (1990) A zoologist looks a humankind. Judith Schaefer, Translator. Columbia University Press. (Originally published in 1944)

    Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces by infants: A preference for female. Perception, 31, 1109–1121.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redshaw, J., Nielsen, M., Slaughter, V., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Oostenbroek, J., Crimston, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2019). Individual differences in neonatal ‘imitation’ fail to predict early social cognitive behavior. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12892.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, H. A., & Birch, H. G. (1974). Infant visual fixation: The effect of concentricity, curvilinearity, and number of directions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 17, 460–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salapatek, P., & Kessen, W. (1966). Visual scanning of triangles by the human newborn. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 155–167.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spelker, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10, 89–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tham, D. S. Y., Bremner, J. G., & Hay, D. (2017). The other-race effect in children from a multiracial population: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 155, 128–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Belknap Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaish, A., & Striano, T. (2004). Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds’ social referencing behavior. Developmental Science, 7, 261–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volk, A. A., & Atkinson, J. A. (2013). Infant and child death in the human environment of evolutionary adaptation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 182–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warneken, F., Gräfenhain, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Collaborative partner or social tool? New evidence for young children’s understanding of joint intentions in collaborative activities. Developmental Science, 15, 54–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301–1303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior & Development, 7, 49–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Xiao, N. G., Quinn, P. C., Liu, S., Ge, L., Pascalis, O., & Lee, K. (2018). Older but not younger infants associate own-race faces with happy music and other-race faces with sad music. Developmental Science, 21, e12537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David F. Bjorklund .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Bjorklund, D.F., Hart, S.L. (2022). Infancy Through the Lens of Evolutionary Developmental Science. In: Hart, S.L., Bjorklund, D.F. (eds) Evolutionary Perspectives on Infancy. Evolutionary Psychology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-76000-7_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics