Skip to main content

Perceiving “Natural” Environments: An Ecological Perspective with Reflections on the Chapters

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Nature and Psychology

Part of the book series: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation ((NSM,volume 67))

Abstract

The way that psychologists conceptualize the nature of visual perception directly influences the way they conceptualize the environment from a psychological standpoint. For that reason, commentary on the contributions to this symposium volume addressing the relationship between the natural environment and psychological functioning is organized with regard to the implicit approach to visual perception each chapter adopts. Such theoretical commitments shape how we think about the environment from a psychological perspective, the kind of research questions we ask, the particular methodologies that are employed and, in some cases, the practical applications that stem from them. This examination of the volume’s chapters is preceded by a discussion of the complexities attending a definition of the natural environment, and a proposal that psychologists might base that definition on those environmental properties that perceivers attend to when designating an environment as natural. Having specified those perceivable properties, we can proceed to inquire on what basis perceivers express greater preference for natural environments. In the course of these reflections, other pertinent issues are discussed, including environmental preference in relation to evolutionary theory; affordances and environmental preference; and psychological restoration as a process of re-establishing functional stability. Particular attention is paid to the place of perception-action in relation to the environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Altman, I., & Rogoff, B. (1987). World views in psychology: Trait, interactional, organismic, and transactional perspectives. In D. Stokols & I. Altman (Eds.), Handbook of environmental psychology (pp. 7–40). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnberger, A., & Eder, R. (2012). The influence of green space on community attachment of urban and suburban residents. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 11, 41–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 84–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, M., Hout, M., Kardan, O., Hunter, M. R., Yourganov, G., Henderson, J., et al. (2014). The perception of naturalness correlates with low-level features of environmental scenes. PLoS One, 9(12), e114572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, M., Jonides, J., & Kaplan, S. (2008). The cognitive benefits of interacting with nature. Psychological Science, 19, 1207–1212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. (2015). Developing adaptations. Developmental Review, 38, 13–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Evolutionary psychology and child development: An emerging synthesis. In B. J. Ellis & D. F. Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and child development (pp. 3–18). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The origins of human nature: Evolutionary developmental psychology. American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (2007). What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121(3), 227–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouton, M. E., & Nelson, J. B. (1998). The role of context in classical conditioning: Some implications for cognitive behavior therapy. In W. O’Donohue (Ed.), Learning and behavior therapy (pp. 59–84). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caldicott, J. B., & Nelson, M. P. (Eds.). (1998). The great new wilderness debate. Athens: University of Georgia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chawla, L. T. (1993). Childhood place attachments. In I. Altman & S. M. Low (Eds.), Place attachment: Human Behavior and environment (Vol. 12, pp. 63–86). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chawla, L. T. (1994). In the country of first places: Nature, poetry, and childhood memory. Binghampton, NY: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chawla, L. T. (2015). The benefits of nature contact for children. Journal of Planning Literature, 30, 433–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chawla, L. T., & Derr, V. (2012). The development of conservation behaviors in childhood and youth. In S. Clayton (Ed.), Handbook of environmental and conservation psychology (pp. 27–55). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chawla, L., & Heft, H. (2002). Children’s competence and the ecology of communities: A functional approach to the evaluation of participation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22, 201–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2014). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronon, W. (1996, Ed.). Uncommon ground: Rethinking the human place in nature. New York: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1998). Cultural psychology: The once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, K. (1973). Environmental psychology. Annual Reviews in Psychology, 24, 403–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crossley, M. (2000). Introducing narrative psychology. Philadelphia: Open Court. ISBN 0 335 20291 8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3, 357–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1925). Experience and nature. Chicago: Open Court Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • DiPaolo, E. A., Buhrmann, T., & Barandiaran, X. E. (2017). Sensorimotor life: An enactive proposal. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H., & Taylor, C. (2015). Retrieving realism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Egeth, H. E., Leonard, C. J., & Leber, A. B. (2010). Why salience is not enough: Reflections on top-down selection in vision. Acta Psychologica, 135, 130–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, L. E. (2016). Exploring the restorative effects of environments through conditioning. The conditioned restoration theory. Master’s thesis, Lillehammer University College, Lillehammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. American Psychologist, 59, 77–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1960). The concept of the stimulus in psychology. American Psychologist, 15(11), 694–703.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1967). New reasons for realism. Synthese, 17, 162–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, E. B. (2009). Encyclopedia of Perception. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (1987). The developmental basis of evolutionary change. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 101, 262–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.101.3.262

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, P. E., & Gray, R. D. (1994). Developmental systems and evolutionary explanation. Journal of Philosophy, 91, 277–304. https://doi.org/10.2307/2940982

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlyn, D. (1961). Sensation and perception: A history of philosophy of perception. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, C. (1963). Adaptation to displaced vision: Visual, motor, or proprioceptive change? Science, 140, 812–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, R. (1978). Children’s experience of place: A developmental study. New York: Irvington Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartig, T., Catalano, R., Ong, M., & Syme, S. L. (2013). Vacation, collective restoration, and mental health in a population. Society and Mental Health, 3, 221–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatfield, G. (1990). The natural and the normative: Theories of spatial perception from Kant to Helmholtz. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermuller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (1988). Joachim F. Wohlwill (1928-1987): His contributions to the emerging discipline of environmental psychology. Environment and Behavior, 20, 259–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (1989). Affordances and the body: An intentional analysis of Gibson’s ecological approach to visual perception. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 19, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (1998). Towards a functional ecology of behavior and development: The legacy of Joachim F. Wohlwill. In D. Gorlitz, H. J. Harloff, G. Mey, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Children, cities, and psychological theories: Developing relationships (pp. 85–110). Berlin: Walter De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2001). Ecological psychology in context: James Gibson, Roger Barker, and the legacy of William James’s radical empiricism. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2003). Affordances, dynamic experience, and the challenge of reification. Ecological Psychology, 15, 149–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2007). The social constitution of perceiver-environment reciprocity. Ecological Psychology, 19, 85–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2018). Places: Widening the scope of an ecological approach to perception-action with an emphasis on child development. Ecological Psychology, 30, 99–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2019). What’s wrong with using photographs of the environment in environmental perception research? In A. Beth, R. Wener, E. Yoon, R. A. Rae, & J. Morris (Eds.), Proceedings from EDRA 50: Sustainable urban environments. Brooklyn, NY: Environmental Design Research Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heft, H. (2020). Revisiting the discovery of the occluding edge and its implications for perception 40 years on. In J. Wagman & J. Blau (Eds.), Perception as information detection: Reflections on Gibson’s ecological approach to visual perception (pp. 188–204). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Held, R., & Hein, A. (1963). Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 872–876.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holton, M. T., Dieterlen, S. L., & Sullivan, W. C. (2015). Social life under cover: Tree canopy and social capital in Baltimore, Maryland. Environment and Behavior, 47, 502–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, A., Sallis, J. F., King, A. C., Conway, T. L., Saelens, B., Cain, K. L., et al. (2018). Linking green space to neighborhood social capital in older adults; The role of perceived safety. Social Science & Medicine, 207, 38–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopstaken, J. F., van der Linden, D., Kompier, M., & Bakker, A. B. (2016). Shifts in attention during mental fatigue: Evidence from subjective, behavioral, physiological, and eye-tracking data. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 878–889.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1878). Remarks on Spencer’s definition of mind as correspondence. Journal of Speculative Philosophy, 12, 1–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1892). Psychology (Briefer course). New York: Henry Holt.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1904). Does consciousness exist? Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 1, 477–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joyce, P. (1988). Hockney on photography. New York: Harmony Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joye, Y., & Van den Berg, A. E. (2011). Is love for green in our genes? A critical analysis of evolutionary assumptions in restorative environments research. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 10(4), 261–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ufug.2011.07.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: Intentional behavior as a complex system. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jungnickel, E., & Grammann, K. (2016). Mobile brain/body imaging (MoBI) of physical interaction with dynamically moving objects. Frontiers Human Neuroscience, 27. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00306

  • Kaplan, S. (1972). The challenge of environmental psychology: A proposal for a new functionalism. American Psychologist, 27, 140–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 15(3), 169–182. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-4944(95)90001-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, S., & Berman, M. G. (2010). Directed attention as a common resource for executive functioning and self-regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 43–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karden, O., Demiralp, E., Karimi, H., Hout, M., Hunter, M. R., Yourganov, G., et al. (2015). Is the preference for natural versus man-made scenes driven by bottom-up processing of visual features? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 471. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00471

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, E. F. (2010). The mirage of a space between nature and nurture. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kellert, S. (2008). Dimensions, elements, and attributes of biophilia design. In S. R. Kellert, J. H. Heerwagen, & M. L. Mador (Eds.), Biophilic design: The theory, science, and practice of bringing buildings to life (pp. 3–20). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kossyln, S. M., Ganis, G., & Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2, 635–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krieger, M. H. (1973). What’s wrong with plastic trees? Science, 179, 446–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1969). Theories of development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R. (2018). Concepts and theories in human development (4th ed.). New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lerstrup, I. (2015). Green settings for children in Preschool: Affordance-based considerations for design and management. Ph.D. thesis, Faculty of Science, University of Copenhagen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerstrup, I., & Moller, M. S. (2016). Affordances of ditches for preschool children. Children, Youth and Environments, 26, 43–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1943). Defining the “field at a given time”. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Field theory in social science: Selected theoretical papers (pp. 43–59). New York: Harper.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L. S., Patterson, A. H., & Newcombe, N. (Eds.). (1981). Spatial representation and behavior across the life span. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lickliter, R. (2008). The growth of developmental thought: Implications for a new evolutionary psychology. New Ideas in Psychology, 26, 353–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, W., & Heft, H. (2010). Ecological psychology. In B. Goldstein (Ed.), Encyclopedia of perception. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mark, L. S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 361–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClellend, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1988). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEwen, B. S. (2009). The brain is the central organ of stress and adaptation. NeuroImage, 4, 911–913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyers, G. E. (1986). William James, His life and thought. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. (1985). The constitutive problem of psychology. In S. Koch & D. E. Leary (Eds.), A century of psychology as a science (pp. 40–450). New York: MacGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rhinehart, & Winston.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, G. (1976). Theory and research on the development of environmental knowing. In G. T. Mooore & R. G. Golledge (Eds.), Environmental knowing. Theories, research, and methods (pp. 138–164). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, M. (2011). Creating capabilities: The human development approach. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, S. (1985). The ontogeny of information: Developmental system and evolution. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, S. E. (1999). Vision science: Photons to phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pastore, N. (1971). Selective history of theories of visual perception 1650 – 1950. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepper, S. (1942). World hypotheses: A study in evidence. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H., Johnston, J. C., & Ruthroff, E. (2001). Attention and performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 629–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, M. A. (1999). High-level vision. In R. W. Wilson & F. C. Keil (Eds.), The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences (pp. 374–376). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge: An essay on the relations between organic regulations and cognitive processes. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M., & Petersen, S. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proshansky, H., Ittelson, W., & Rivlin, L. (1970). Environmental psychology: Man and his physical setting. New York: Holt, Rhinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purcell, T., Peron, E., & Berto, R. (2001). Why do preferences differ between scene types? Environment and Behavior, 33, 93–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S. (1996a). Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, E. S. (1996b). The necessity of experience. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, E. (2008). The skillful body as a concernful system of possible actions: Phenomena and neurodynamics. Theory & Psychology, 18, 341–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, E., & Kiverstein, J. (2014). A rich landscape of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 26, 325–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, E., Denys, D., & van Westen, M. (2020). Ecological-enactive cognition as engaging a field of relevant affordances: The skilled intentionality framework. In A. Newn, L. de Bruin, & S. Gallagher (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of 4E cognition (pp. 41–70). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolston, H. (1997). Nature for real: Is nature a social construct? In T. Chappell (Ed.), The philosophy of the environment (pp. 38–64). Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, R. (2000). Essays on life itself. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarbin, T. R. (1986). Narrative psychology: The storied nature of human conduct. London: Praeger. ISBN 9780275921033.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schertz, K. E., Sachdeva, S., Kardan, O., Kotabe, H. P., Wolf, K., & Berman, M. G. (2018). A thought in the park. The influence of naturalism and low-level visual features on expressed thoughts. Cognition, 174, 82–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sen, A. (1985). Commodities and capabilities. Amsterdam, NY; New York, NY: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. (2014). The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition. New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, J., & Allen, N. W. (1975). Photography, vision, and representation. Critical Inquiry, 2, 143–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soper, K. (1995). What is nature? Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E., Hirst, W., & Neisser, U. (1976). Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4, 215–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stokols, D., & Altman, I. (Eds.). (1987). Handbook of environmental psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T. (2019). Lectures on perception: An ecological approach. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R. S. (1983). Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human behavior and environment: Advances in theory and research (Vol. 6, pp. 85–125). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R. S. (2008). Biophilic theory and research for healthcare design. In S. R. Kellert, J. H. Heerwagen, & M. L. Mador (Eds.), Biophilic design: The theory, science, and practice of bringing buildings to life (pp. 87–106). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Berg, A. E., Joye, Y., & Koole, S. L. (2016). Why viewing nature is more fascinating and restorative than viewing buildings: A closer look at perceived complexity. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 20, 397–401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ufug.2016.10.011

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, S. (1996). Against nature: The concept of nature in critical theory. Binghamton, NY: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vogel, S. (2015). Thinking like a mall: Environmental philosophy after the end of nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wachs, T. D. (2000). Necessary but not sufficient. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wachs, T. D., & Gruen, G. (1982). Early experience and human development. New York: Plenum Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, D. (2015). Organisms, agency, and evolution. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair-climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welch, R. B. (1969). Adaptation to prism-displacement vision: The importance of target-pointing. Perception & Psychophysics, 5, 305–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R., & Keil, F. (Eds.). (1999). The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witherington, D. C., & Lickliter, R. (2017). Integrating development and evolution in psychological science: Evolutionary developmental psychology, developmental systems, and explanatory pluralism. Human Development, 59, 200–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Withagen, R., Araujo, D., & de Poel, H. (2017). Inviting affordances and agency. New Ideas in Psychology, 45, 11–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1970). The emerging discipline of environmental psychology. American Psychologist, 25(4), 303–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1973a). The concept of experience: S or R? Human Development, 16, 90–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1973b). The environment is not in the head! In W. F. E. Preiser (Ed.), Environmental design research (Vol. 1, pp. 166–181). Stroudsburg, PA: Dowden, Hutchinson, & Ross.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1976). Environmental aesthetics: The environment as a source of affect. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human behavior and Environment (Vol. 1, pp. 37–86). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wohlwill, J. F. (1983). The concept of nature: A psychologist’s view. In I. Altman & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human behavior and environment (Vol. 6, pp. 5–37). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to the editors of this volume, Anne Schutte, Jeffrey Stevens, and Julia Torquati, for inviting me to participate in the long-esteemed Nebraska Symposium Series, and for their helpful feedback and suggestions on earlier versions of this chapter. I also thank Louise Chawla and Agnes van den Berg for their comments on some sections. Finally, the influence of Jack Wohlwill on my initial thinking about environmental perception and environmental psychology generally should be apparent here, and I dedicate this chapter to his memory.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Harry Heft .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Heft, H. (2021). Perceiving “Natural” Environments: An Ecological Perspective with Reflections on the Chapters. In: Schutte, A.R., Torquati, J.C., Stevens, J.R. (eds) Nature and Psychology. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, vol 67. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-69020-5_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics