Skip to main content

Morality as an Evolutionary Exaptation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Empirically Engaged Evolutionary Ethics

Part of the book series: Synthese Library ((SYLI,volume 437))

Abstract

The dominant theory of the evolution of moral cognition across a variety of fields is that moral cognition is a biological adaptation to foster social cooperation. This chapter argues, to the contrary, that moral cognition is likely an evolutionary exaptation: a form of cognition where neurobiological capacities selected for in our evolutionary history for a variety of different reasons—many unrelated to social cooperation—were put to a new, prosocial use after the fact through individual rationality, learning, and the development and transmission of social norms. This chapter begins with a brief overview of the emerging behavioral neuroscience of moral cognition. It then outlines a novel theory of moral cognition that I have previously argued explains these findings better than alternatives. Finally, it shows how the evidence for this theory of moral cognition and human evolutionary history together suggest that moral cognition is likely not a biological adaptation. Instead, like reading sheet music or riding a bicycle, moral cognition is something that individuals learn to do—in this case, in response to sociocultural norms created in our ancestral history and passed down through the ages to enable cooperative living.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    I do not mean to endorse neuroessentialism here, the view that specific capacities are located in or identical to the functions of particular brain regions. I merely affirm scientific findings that particular brain regions are associated with particular cognitive functions.

  2. 2.

    The following overview of DMN regions is from Arvan (2020), pp. 12–13. As I argue in Arvan (2020), chapter 4, although the DMN is involved in many cognitive tasks other than moral cognition , my account provides a powerful normative and descriptive explanation of why and how some of the main cognitive functions associated with these DMN regions should and do interact to generate moral cognition. Cf. Pascual, Gallardo-Pujol, and Rodrigues (2013); Sommer et al. (2014).

References

  • Acheson, D. J., & Hagoort, P. (2013). Stimulating the brain’s language network: Syntactic ambiguity resolution after TMS to the inferior frontal gyrus and middle temporal gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(10), 1664–1677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. New York: Routledge, 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amunts, K., Kedo, O., Kindler, M., Pieperhoff, P., Mohlberg, H., Shah, N. J., et al. (2005). Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and entorhinal cortex: Intersubject variability and probability maps. Anatomy and Embryology, 210(5–6), 343–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anand, P. (1995). Foundations of rational choice under risk. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apicella, C. L., & Silk, J. B. (2019). The evolution of human cooperation. Current Biology, 29(11), R447–R450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aristotle. (1984). Nicomachean ethics. In J. Barnes (Ed.), The complete works of Aristotle: The revised Oxford translation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arvan, M. (2016). Rightness as fairness: A moral and political theory. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Arvan, M. (2019). The dark side of morality: Group polarization and moral epistemology. The Philosophical Forum, 50(1), 87–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arvan, M. (2020). Neurofunctional prudence and morality: A philosophical theory. New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Audi, R. (2015). Intuition and its place in ethics. Journal of the American Philosophical Association, 1(1), 57–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barbas, H. (2007). Flow of information for emotions through temporal and orbitofrontal pathways. Journal of Anatomy, 211(2), 237–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barceló-Coblijn, L. (2012). Evolutionary scenarios for the emergence of recursion. Theoria et Historia Scientarium, IX, 171–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barragan, R. C., Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2020). Altruistic food sharing behavior by human infants after a hunger manipulation. Scientific Reports, 10(1785). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-58645-9

  • Baskin-Sommers, A., Stuppy-Sullivan, A. M., & Buckholtz, J. W. (2016). Psychopathic individuals exhibit but do not avoid regret during counterfactual decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(50), 14438–14443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123(11), 2189–2202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benoit, R. G., Gilbert, S. J., & Burgess, P. W. (2011). A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(18), 6771–6779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 182(1), 5–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanke, O., Mohr, C., Michel, C. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Brugger, P., Seeck, M., et al. (2005). Linking out-of-body experience and self processing to mental own-body imagery at the temporoparietal junction. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(3), 550–557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boaz, N. T., Ciochon, R. L., Xu, Q., & Liu, J. (2004). Mapping and taphonomic analysis of the Homo erectus loci at Locality 1 Zhoukoudian, China. Journal of Human Evolution, 46(5), 519–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogousslavsky, J., Miklossy, J., Deruaz, J. P., Assal, G., & Regli, F. (1987). Lingual and fusiform gyri in visual processing: A clinico-pathologic study of superior altitudinal hemianopia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 50(5), 607–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonner, M. F., & Price, A. R. (2013). Where is the anterior temporal lobe and what does it do? Journal of Neuroscience, 33(10), 4213–4215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braddock, M., & Rosenberg, A. (2012). Reconstruction in moral philosophy? Analyse & Kritik, 34(1), 63–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, J. (2010). Scepticism about philosophy. Ratio, 23(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bricker, P. (1980). Prudence. The Journal of Philosophy, 77(7), 381–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Nonhuman species’ reactions to inequity and their implications for fairness. Social Justice Research, 19(2), 53–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. (2014). Evolution of responses to (un)fairness. Science, 346(6207), 1251776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruckner, D. (2003). A contractarian account of (part of) prudence. American Philosophical Quarterly, 40(1), 33–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R., Heywood, C. A., Cowey, A., Regard, M., & Landis, T. (1990). Sensitivity to eye gaze in prosopagnosic patients and monkeys with superior temporal sulcus ablation. Neuropsychologia, 28(11), 1123–1142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P., & James, S. M. (2008). Evolution and the possibility of moral realism. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 77(1), 237–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casebeer, W. D. (2003). Natural ethical facts: Evolution, connectionism, and moral cognition. Cambridge, MA/London: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Jones, R. M., & Hare, T. A. (2008). The adolescent brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 111–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland, P. (2011). Braintrust: What neuroscience tells us about morality. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (2007). The uniqueness of human recursive thinking: The ability to think about thinking may be the critical attribute that distinguishes us from all other species. American Scientist, 95(3), 240–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 163–228). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crockford, D. N., Goodyear, B., Edwards, J., Quickfall, J., & el-Guebaly, N. (2005). Cue-induced brain activity in pathological gamblers. Biological Psychiatry, 58(10), 787–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curry, O. S. (2016). Morality as cooperation: A problem-centred approach. In T. K. Shackelford & D. Hansen (Eds.), The evolution of morality (pp. 27–51). New York/Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, H., Grabowski, T., Frank, R., Galaburda, A. M., & Damasio, A. R. (1994). The return of Phineas Gage: Clues about the brain from the skull of a famous patient. Science, 264(5162), 1102–1105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniel, T. O., Stanton, C. M., & Epstein, L. H. (2013). The future is now: Comparing the effect of episodic future thinking on impulsivity in lean and obese individuals. Appetite, 71(1), 120–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, B., Christie, J., & Rorden, C. (2009). Temporal order judgments activate temporal parietal junction. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(10), 3182–3188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. (2006). Primates and philosophers: How morality evolved. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. The Neuroscientist, 13(6), 580–593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doğruyol, B., Alper, S., & Yilmaz, O. (2019). The five-factor model of the moral foundations theory is stable across WEIRD and non-WEIRD cultures. Personality and Individual Differences, 151, 109547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ersner-Hershfield, H., Garton, M. T., Ballard, K., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., & Knutson, B. (2009). Don’t stop thinking about tomorrow: Individual differences in future self-continuity account for saving. Judgment and Decision making, 4, 280–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ersner-Hershfield, H., Wimmer, G. E., & Knutson, B. (2009). Saving for the future self: Neural measures of future self-continuity predict temporal discounting. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(1), 85–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrow, T. F., Ying Zheng, Y., Wilkinson, I. D., Spence, S. A., Deakin, J. F., Tarrier, N., et al. (2001). Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness. Neuroreport, 12(11), 2433–2438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fellows, L. K., & Farah, M. J. (2007). The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in decision making: Judgment under uncertainty or judgment per se? Cerebral Cortex, 17(11), 2669–2674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fettes, P., Schulze, L., & Downar, J. (2017). Cortico-striatal-thalamic loop circuits of the orbitofrontal cortex: Promising therapeutic targets in psychiatric illness. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 11(25), 1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. New York: WW Norton & Co..

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, J. M. (2001). The prefrontal cortex—An update: Time is of the essence. Neuron, 30(2), 319–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, H. L., Happé, F., Brunswick, N., Fletcher, P. C., Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: An fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks. Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 11–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George, N., Dolan, R. J., Fink, G. R., Baylis, G. C., Russell, C., & Driver, J. (1999). Contrast polarity and face recognition in the human fusiform gyrus. Nature Neuroscience, 2(6), 574–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geraci, A., & Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: Infants’ reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Developmental Science, 14, 1012–1020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., & Jeffries, N. O. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2(10), 861–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1991). Exaptation: A crucial tool for an evolutionary psychology. Journal of Social Issues, 47(3), 43–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., et al. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. In Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 55–130). Amsterdam: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D. (2013). Moral tribes: Emotion, reason, and the gap between us and them. New York: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J. D. (2015). The rise of moral cognition. Cognition, 135, 39–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gusnard, D. A., Akbudak, E., Shulman, G. L., & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(7), 4259–4264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133(4), 55–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haldane, M., Cunningham, G., Androutsos, C., & Frangou, S. (2008). Structural brain correlates of response inhibition in Bipolar Disorder I. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 22(2), 138–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hallos, J. (2005). “15 minutes of fame:” Exploring the temporal dimension of Middle Pleistocene lithic technology. Journal of Human Evolution, 49, 155–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamlin, J. K. (2013). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: Evidence for an innate moral core. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(3), 186–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han, H. (2017). Neural correlates of moral sensitivity and moral judgment associated with brain circuitries of selfhood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Moral Education, 46(2), 97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansson, S. O. (2005). Decision theory: A brief introduction. https://people.kth.se/~soh/decisiontheory.pdf. Retrieved 7 December 2020.

  • Hare, R. D. (1999). The Hare psychopathy checklist-revised: PLC-R. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, J. M. (1848). Passage of an iron rod through the head. The Boston Medical and Surgical Journal (1828–1851), 39(20), 0_1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, S. D., & Dempster, R. J. (1997). Impulsivity and psychopathy. In C. D. Webster & M. A. Jackson (Eds.), Impulsivity: Theory, assessment, and treatment (pp. 212–232). New York/London: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haybron, D. (2011). Happiness. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. E. N. Zalta (Ed.). https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2011/entries/happiness/.

  • Hayden, B. Y., & Platt, M. L. (2010). Neurons in anterior cingulate cortex multiplex information about reward and action. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(9), 3339–3346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, N., & Henrich, J. (2007). Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hershfield, H. E., Cohen, T. R., & Thompson, L. (2012). Short horizons and tempting situations: Lack of continuity to our future selves leads to unethical decision making and behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117, 298–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hershfield, H. E., Goldstein, D. G., Sharpe, W. F., Fox, J., Yeykelis, L., Carstensen, L. L., et al. (2011). Increasing saving behavior through age-progressed renderings of the future self. Journal of Marketing Research, 48(SPL), S23–S37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hitler, A. (1925). Mein Kampf. R. Manheim (trans), New York: Houghton Mifflin, 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobbes, T. (1651). Leviathan. In Sir W. Molesworth (Ed.), The English works of Thomas Hobbes: Now first collected and edited (Vol. 3, p. ix-714). London: John Bohn, 1839-45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobbes, T. (1658). De Homine. In B. Gert (Ed.), Man and citizen. Anchor Books, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hosking, J. G., Kastman, E. K., Dorfman, H. M., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Baskin-Sommers, A., Kiehl, K. A., et al. (2017). Disrupted prefrontal regulation of striatal subjective value signals in psychopathy. Neuron, 95(1), 221–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isoda, M., & Noritake, A. (2013). What makes the dorsomedial frontal cortex active during reading the mental states of others? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 887–900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joyce, R. (2007). The myth of morality. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kabadayi, C., & Osvath, M. (2017). Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science, 357(6347), 202–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 4, 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. (1785). Groundwork of the metaphysics of morals. In M. J. Gregor (Ed.), The Cambridge edition of the works of Immanuel Kant: Practical philosophy (pp. 38–108). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennett, J., & Matthews, S. (2009). Mental timetravel, agency and responsibility. In M. Broome & L. Bortolotti (Eds.), Psychiatry as cognitive neuroscience: Philosophical perspectives (pp. 327–350). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher, P. (1998). Psychological altruism, evolutionary origins, and moral rules. Philosophical Studies, 89(2–3), 283–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher, P. (2005). Biology and ethics. In D. Copp (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of ethical theory (pp. 163–185). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher, P. (2011). The ethical project. Cambridge, UK: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kjaer, T. W., Nowak, M., & Lou, H. C. (2002). Reflective self-awareness and conscious states: PET evidence for a common midline parietofrontal core. NeuroImage, 17(2), 1080–1086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., et al. (2007). Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature, 446(7138), 908–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korsgaard, C. M. (2008). The constitution of agency. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Korsgaard, C. M. (2009). Self-constitution: Agency, identity, and integrity. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kringelbach, M. L., & Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology, 72(5), 341–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambon Ralph, M. A., Pobric, G., & Jefferies, E. (2008). Conceptual knowledge is underpinned by the temporal pole bilaterally: Convergent evidence from rTMS. Cerebral Cortex, 19(4), 832–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, N. (2007). The responsibility of the psychopath revisited. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology, 14(2), 129–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Litton, P. (2008). Responsibility status of the psychopath: On moral reasoning and rational self-governance. Rutgers Law Journal, 39(349), 350–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lou, H. C., Luber, B., Crupain, M., Keenan, J. P., Nowak, M., Kjaer, T. W., et al. (2004). Parietal cortex and representation of the mental self. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101(17), 6827–6832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luco, A. C. (2016). Non-negotiable: Why moral naturalism cannot do away with categorical reasons. Philosophical Studies, 173(9), 2511–2528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundstrom, B. N., Petersson, K. M., Andersson, J., Johansson, M., Fransson, P., & Ingvar, M. (2003). Isolating the retrieval of imagined pictures during episodic memory: Activation of the left precuneus and left prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage, 20(4), 1934–1943.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maren, S. (1999). Long-term potentiation in the amygdala: A mechanism for emotional learning and memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 22(12), 561–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, S. E., Lau, M. Y., & Howard, G. S. (2015). Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? American Psychologist, 70(6), 487–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • May, J. (2018). Regard for reason in the moral mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 293–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Belger, A., & Allison, T. (1999). Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. II: Response properties of face-specific potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 9(5), 431–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meadows, J. C. (1974). The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 37(5), 489–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milad, M. R., Quinn, B. T., Pitman, R. K., Orr, S. P., Fischl, B., & Rauch, S. L. (2005). Thickness of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in humans is correlated with extinction memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(30), 10706–10711.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millikan, R. G. (1989). In defense of proper functions. Philosophy of Science, 56(2), 288–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2693–2698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Motzkin, J. C., Newman, J. P., Kiehl, K. A., & Koenigs, M. (2011). Reduced prefrontal connectivity in psychopathy. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(48), 17348–17357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieh, E. H., Kim, S. Y., Namburi, P., & Tye, K. M. (2013). Optogenetic dissection of neural circuits underlying emotional valence and motivated behaviors. Brain Research, 1511, 73–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, I. R., Plotzker, A., & Ezzyat, Y. (2007). The enigmatic temporal pole: A review of findings on social and emotional processing. Brain, 130(7), 1718–1731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parfit, D. (2011). On what matters (Vol. Vols. 1&2). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pascual, L., Gallardo-Pujol, D., & Rodrigues, P. (2013). How does morality work in the brain? A functional and structural perspective of moral behavior. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7(65), 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paton, J. J., Belova, M. A., Morrison, S. E., & Salzman, C. D. (2006). The primate amygdala represents the positive and negative value of visual stimuli during learning. Nature, 439(7078), 865–870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peters, J., & Büchel, C. (2010). Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron, 66(1), 138–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, M. (2017). An introduction to decision theory (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pettigrew, R. (2020). Choosing for changing selves. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, B. W. (2002). The worthwhileness theory of the prudentially rational life. Journal of Philosophical Research, 27, 619–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prichard, H. A. (1912). Does moral philosophy rest on a mistake? Mind, 21(81), 21–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. J. (2007). The emotional construction of morals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purves, D., Augustine, G. J., Fitzpatrick, D., Hall, W. C., LaMantia, A., McNamara, J. O., et al. (2001). Neuroscience (2nd ed.). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renier, L. A., Anurova, I., De Volder, A. G., Carlson, S., VanMeter, J., & Rauschecker, J. P. (2010). Preserved functional specialization for spatial processing in the middle occipital gyrus of the early blind. Neuron, 68(1), 138–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, W. D. (1930). The right and the good. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scanlon, T. M. (1998). What we owe to each other. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scanlon, T. M. (2014). Being realistic about reasons. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, M. F., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PLoS One, 6(10), e23223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seghier, M. L. (2013). The angular gyrus: Multiple functions and multiple subdivisions. The Neuroscientist, 19(1), 43–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shergill, S. S., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Williams, S. C. R., Murray, R. M., & McGuire, P. K. (2001). A functional study of auditory verbal imagery. Psychological Medicine, 31(2), 241–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shoemaker, D. W. (2011). Psychopathy, responsibility, and the moral/conventional distinction. Southern Journal of Philosophy, 49(s1), 99–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, N. (2012). Metaethics, teleosemantics and the function of moral judgments. Biology and Philosophy, 27(5), 639–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 81–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., & Tusche, A. (2014). Understanding others: Brain mechanisms of theory of mind and empathy. In P. W. Glimcher & E. Fehr (Eds.), Neuroeconomics: Decision making and the brain (2nd ed., pp. 249–266). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smyth, N. (2017). The function of morality. Philosophical Studies, 174(5), 1127–1144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sommer, M., Meinhardt, J., Rothmayr, C., Döhnel, K., Hajak, G., Rupprecht, R., et al. (2014). Me or you? Neural correlates of moral reasoning in everyday conflict situations in adolescents and adults. Social Neuroscience, 9(5), 452–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soutschek, A., Ruff, C. C., Strombach, T., Kalenscher, T., & Tobler, P. N. (2016). Brain stimulation reveals crucial role of overcoming self-centeredness in self-control. Science Advances, 2(10), e1600992.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiridon, M., Fischl, B., & Kanwisher, N. (2006). Location and spatial profile of category-specific regions in human extrastriate cortex. Human Brain Mapping, 27(1), 77–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K., & Fraser, B. (2016). Evolution and moral realism. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 68(4), 981–1006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss, D. T., Gow, C. A., & Hetherington, C. R. (1992). ‘No longer gage’: Frontal lobe dysfunction and emotional changes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60(3), 349–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suhler, C. L., & Churchland, P. (2011). Can innate, modular “foundations” explain morality? Challenges for Haidt’s moral foundations theory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2103–2116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 231–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tosi, J., & Warmke, B. (2016). Moral Grandstanding. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 44(3), 197–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trueman, C. N. (2020). The Fuhrer Principle. History Learning Site. https://www.historylearningsite.co.uk/nazi-germany/the-fuehrer-principle/. Retrieved 7 December 2020.

  • Van Gelder, J. L., Hershfield, H. E., & Nordgren, L. F. (2013). Vividness of the future self predicts delinquency. Psychological Science, 24(6), 974–980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viganò, E. (2017). Adam Smith’s theory of prudence updated with neuroscientific and behavioral evidence. Neuroethics, 10(2), 215–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogeley, K., May, M., Ritzl, A., Falkai, P., Zilles, K., & Fink, G. R. (2004). Neural correlates of first-person perspective as one constituent of human self-consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(5), 817–827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, B. A., & Laureys, S. (2005). Posterior cingulate, precuneal and retrosplenial cortices: Cytology and components of the neural network correlates of consciousness. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 205–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walton, M. E., Behrens, T. E., Buckley, M. J., Rudebeck, P. H., & Rushworth, M. F. (2010). Separable learning systems in the macaque brain and the role of orbitofrontal cortex in contingent learning. Neuron, 65(6), 927–939.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, S., Habel, U., Amunts, K., & Schnieder, F. (2008). Structural brain abnormalities in psychopaths—A review. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 26(1), 7–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wisdom, J. (2017). Proper-function moral realism. European Journal of Philosophy, 25(4), 1660–1674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, A. (2008). Kantian ethics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yang, Y., & Raine, A. (2009). Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 174(2), 81–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marcus Arvan .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Arvan, M. (2021). Morality as an Evolutionary Exaptation. In: De Smedt, J., De Cruz, H. (eds) Empirically Engaged Evolutionary Ethics. Synthese Library, vol 437. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-68802-8_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics