Keywords

1 Introduction

Research collaboration is the foundation of research students’ efforts in academia. Independently of disciplinary background, research is based on the social patterns of competition for the best explanation and joint evaluation of the quality of research. Therefore, research collaboration is a form of positive interaction between knowledge producers that have taken on management roles by using certain resources and tools to establish and pursue a scientific goal (Ynalvez et al. 2011). We define research collaboration as the current and future regulations, processes, and concepts which support interaction and cooperation between our doctoral candidates. Here, it is important to note that collaboration is not simply students and professors co-authoring a piece of research; instead, it requires establishing connections that might extend to communication which, over time, develops into sustainable collaboration among different researchers with similar interests. Accordingly, we may need to better understand the nature of scientific tasks and the time frame in which they should be completed, as well as how individual beliefs of using ICT and web 2.0 in a research context can help to define how online activities should be organised. In addition, the use of technology can be interpreted in relation to cultural contexts and disciplines. Finally, incentives act as the engine that encourages students to undertake collaborative research, and, in academia, this engine is covered and protected by research ethics. In this paper, we focus on collaboration of all PhD students in their first, second, or third year. This may take into consideration the form of any formal or informal social action and scientific activities that could increase the output and production of scholarly research, improve communication through the text, and encourage resource sharing and collaborative writing.

PhD students face new challenges in the age of digital research. In particular, this paper focuses on challenges such as dealing with digital material and resources, learning management systems, personal learning environments, social networks, and collaboration in research networks. Current PhD students, who are largely from the Generation Y demographic group (born between 1982 and 2000), are familiar with technology and are likely to encounter one or more web 2.0 technologies in their everyday life (Zaman 2010). In the academic context, web 2.0 technology shapes how PhD students learn, self-regulate, and communicate. Accordingly, universities have begun to use and provide these facilities of infrastructure to attract and connect students and develop—step by step—a better practice for research collaboration. However, as Zaman (2010) reports, current doctoral programmes struggle to follow up and meet these demands and requirements. Concerning social and scientific interaction and collaboration among our doctoral students, Mohamed et al. (2013) investigated PhD students’ attitudes towards doctoral colloquium, online learning material via Edu-tech,Footnote 1 and learning management systems via OPAL.Footnote 2 These scientific activities were used simply to provide an informative website for learning material and scientific events; PhD candidates usually found that the community of practice and the feeling of belonging were lacking.

We expect the digital form of research so-called e-research collaboration to comprise the attempt to enhance and develop not only scientific activities such as co-authorship or finding peers and peer reviewers, but what we refer to as “open-kitchen research”. This term refers to sharing research activities not only as a finished product, but also as processes. In fact, during the doctoral candidate education, they attempt to communicate and collaborate only in the context of theoretical curriculum. These learning formal courses are traditionally designed to provide students with only structured theoretical knowledge but no real practices. In most cases, we observed that part-time PhD students working in third-party projects at our laboratory give priority than ever before to the projects they are working in where there is more community support than working individually with their own dissertation.

The relevance of this study can be confirmed by the fact that doctoral education in Germany is rapidly growing in all academic disciplines, to a recent total number of 200,400 doctoral candidates being supervised at German universities (in the winter semester 2010/2011), while only half of this group (n = 104,000) was officially registered (Forschung & Lehre 2012; Wolters and Schmiedel 2010). How do those registered scholars participate in research activities? Do they follow their academic activities at the same pattern and do they regularly use the same research online tools? We can just guess that the new openness of social media and web 2.0 communication helps to provide similar conditions and borderless collaboration for all scholars depending on their access to the Internet. In the German Federal State of Saxony, where the data of this study was collected, the number of PhD degrees has increased more than tenfold, from n = 111 in 1993 to n = 1,206 in 2009 (Saxony State, Statistical Branch 2009).

In order to provide an adequate statement about how our novice researchers collaborate via using web 2.0 services, we explore which factors might shape this collaboration, particularly the collaborative opportunities offered by web 2.0, we begin by developing a theoretical framework for our investigation, and apply it to the current situation of PhD students in Germany.

2 The Fish Model: A Conceptual Framework for E-Research Collaboration

The authors conceptualised e-research collaboration as follows. Based on a meta-analysis, approximately 200 papers focussing on different aspects and approaches in e-science and e-humanities were recruited, organised, and analysed, in order to formulate a proposed conceptual framework, the fish model, previously published in Mohamed et al. (2013). The framework may be used to deepen our understanding of the daily scientific tasks, activities, technologies, and incentives that shape everyday academic practices for doctoral scholars, regardless of their disciplinary heritage. Databases consulted include Science Direct, Pro-quest, EBSCO, Scirus, and Mendeley. Inclusion criteria were limited to full-text papers concerning the use of web 2.0 in research communication and collaboration. Keywords used for collecting scientific articles directly from the mentioned databases included the following: researchers’ digital habits, use of web 2.0 in research, e-research, social media in research, research collaboration, and scholarly communication. The following selection criteria were used for papers: (1) written in English, (2) situated only on the PhD and researcher levels, (3) either empirical or review articles only. In addition, a conceptual definition of collaboration factors from Patel et al. (2011) and the Folk Model of Intentionality (DeAndrea 2012) were used as guides to identify the fish model (Ringle et al. 2005). The first step in analysing the selected papers was to interpret online research behaviours and the academic activities associated with using web 2.0 technologies, in order to predict the future of research collaboration, using the Fish Model (Mohamed et al. 2013). As the model clarifies the factors and concepts behind the best practices associated with research collaboration using web 2.0 technologies, it was proposed to develop an understanding of daily scientific research tasks and activities.

As the authors suggested earlier, online research behaviour is controlled by some key factors and indicators, which was first framed in the Model of Collaborative e-Research (Reebs 2011). This model can be used to describe the factors that support online collaboration in e-science. The fish model (Mohamed et al. 2013), however, extends this research by giving evidence that individual factors (beliefs, self-regulation, etc.), in addition to group interaction organised by the institution, and time management, obviously influence the active production of research, communication among researchers, and subsequent collaboration. Using the fish model, the core factors in online research behaviours and the academic activities associated with using web 2.0 technologies all were investigated.

It is argued that a doctoral scholar would behave “like a fish living in a specific environment, taking part in a particular community, showing different individual behaviours to respond to an action, led by their own beliefs and framed by a certain culture” (Mohamed et al. 2013, p. 3275). Typical behaviours and activities are managed by incentives related to the qualification addressed and controlled by the scholar’s role in the research ecology. The fish metaphor emerged when framing a body of collaboration patterns for the authors’ previous study (Frewox 2010). “Collaboration in research is managed by a dorsal fin to stabilise research against rolling and protect scientific environment from isolation and weakness. Inhalation through the mouth passes over the gills in fish to obtain fresh oxygen, communication is the oxygen of research project which is necessary for bringing activities and ideas to the project and achieve the tasks related. The backbone of our fish is web 2.0 technologies which connect and facilitate all functions of the whole body, these functions are divided concerning a dichotomous aspect (fish spine) – as we will describe it complementarily in the frame of this paper – in a task/time, activities/beliefs, support/context, and ethics/incentives division” (Tannen 2006, p. 3267 ff.).

Research collaboration is usually considered as a planned activity where knowledge can be produced and transferred. The authors predicted previously (Mohamed et al. 2013) that collaborative e-research (using web 2.0 technology to improve best research practices) will take place alongside dichotomies. Tannen (Wang 2010), in his book, The Argument Culture (1998), proposes the concept of perceived dichotomies, that is, binarisms between two connected concepts, while not distinguishing between them through the use of vocabulary such as “good” and “bad”. Building on Tannen’s work, the fish model proposes the integration of both factors. Research collaboration in this study can be interpreted as a relationship between eight concepts formed in pars making up the total of four groups: (a) between scientific tasks or candidates’ needs and time available for implementing them; (b) between planned activities and individual research beliefs in dealing with these activities; (c) support from technology and understanding the uses of this technology within a certain context and culture of an institution; and (d) intentions/motivations for collaboration, which are directed by research ethics, as illustrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
A fish model illustrates the dichotomy for the collaborative e-research. The tail represents ethics, the gills represent the relationship between concepts, and the wing reads collaboration.

Fish model: conceptual framework for developing e-research collaboration for PhD students and novice researchers (Mohamed et al. 2013)

2.1 The Reality of Managing Scientific Tasks in Terms of the Available Time

It can be expected that novice researchers are likely to collaborate and work with each other because they are more likely than experienced researchers to break their work down into various tasks, activities, and actions. Such individual behaviour is controlled by time management as short-/long-term academic tasks, primarily related to different actions such as information search, data analysis, reading, or possibly writing (Illeris 2004). Overall, the doctoral education system differs significantly from programmes at masters and bachelor level, as doctoral programmes prepare candidates for high-level careers in industry or provide long practical experience (Zaman 2010). In their previous studies (Mohamed et al. 2013; Mohamed et al. 2013), the authors identified two key tasks that doctoral students undertake in order to carry out their research. The first is marketing, that is, building a scientific competence profile in order to develop a scientific reputation. The second is doing research, that is activities in daily research practice, including mainly reading, writing, investigating, searching, and reviewing.

H1::

Novice researchers are more likely to collaborate and work with each other when the work task (types, stages, and technologies) and timeframe are specified.

H1-0::

An academic task to be done via web 2.0 is driven by a timeframe (when the task should be done/how much time is needed to do it).

H1-1::

An appropriate timeframe for a task to be carried out via web 2.0 can lead to academic collaboration

2.2 Online Research Activities Led by Work-Based Beliefs

PhD students’ daily research activities include specific online activities, as identified previously (Mohamed et al. 2013): accessing resources, information, and research funds; engagement in scientific discussions and being an active member in one or more academic communities of practice; communication in reviewing, sharing, and exchanging ideas; awareness of recently published scientific papers and events; presenting oneself online in social media and social networking in order to build up a profile and identification (Mohamed 2011; Lahenius 2010; Peggy and Borkowski 2007).

Typically, it is expected that PhD research work is completed through three main development phases (Terrell et al. 2009; Zaman 2010; Mohamed et al. 2013): (a) becoming a researcher by training, and reading activities for first-year PhD students; (b) becoming an expert in any required methods and the pressure to start publishing for second-year PhD students; and (c) becoming an author which includes participating in peer reviewing, co-authoring, and writing publications. Each of those phases requires a number of planned online activities. Additionally, gradual engagement with the literature of one’s own scientific discipline should be considered, because it leads to particular work beliefs. Three main explanations for scholars’ success were identified (Patel et al. 2011): social culture, the culture of disciplines, and the individual beliefs (values, motivation, learning style, self-regulation, cognitive competence, confidence, and trust). Usually, beliefs are addressed by psycho-educational research, whereas the role of trust (versus control) as a governance concept has been addressed in earlier research on virtual organisations (Lattemann and Köhler 2005). Only the combination of these accepted beliefs defines a researcher’s individual approach to scientific activities.

H2::

Novice researchers are more likely to collaborate and work with each other when they believe in the work and participate in academic and research activities (online).

H2-0::

Academic activities (online) affect a researcher’s belief in using web 2.0 toward collaboration.

H2-1::

Researchers’ belief in using web 2.0 for research may increase their chances of collaboration

2.3 Support for Technology Use in Context

Even though web 2.0 is a rather young technology, multiple studies have investigated its benefits for learning, especially in the production and communication of scientific research, or e-science (Pscheida et al. 2013; Kahnwald et al. 2015). A core aspect of ICT infrastructure (web 2.0) is its strong linkage to the sociocultural context and the disciplinary culture. While academic work triggers social interactions among PhD scholars, the cultural context drives and assists their use of web 2.0 technologies in order to interact. ICT and web 2.0 services in learning and research comprise all methods, techniques, online behaviours of scientists, tools used by researchers, knowledge sharing and transfer, acceptance/adoption, and building social networks via e-research identified by literature reviews (Meyer and McNeal 2011). A doctoral candidate’s use of web 2.0 technologies is both supported by and understood through institutional context and discipline culture (Pscheida et al. 2013). Those have a particular need for being involved in one or more academic communities on a national or international level in order to share and develop practice successfully, usually realised through web 2.0 services (Veletsianos and Kimmons 2012; Eyman et al. 2009; Illeris 2004; Gillet et al. 2009; Lam 2011).

H3::

Researchers are more likely to collaborate when they have received technical support in their academic context.

H3-0::

web 2.0 technology may enhance research communication, leading to future collaboration.

H3-1::

Research context has a direct influence on collaboration

2.4 Incentives Protected by Research Ethics

PhD candidates need incentives to be strongly engaged in online collaboration (Pidd 2011); these incentives are intrinsic motivation, satisfaction, and reputation. Purely financial motivation is less important, but the motivation should be protected and controlled by research ethics related to the digital environment (Mutula 2010). The issue of trust should be considered by faculty involved in digital research processes (Jirotka et al. 2006), as it has a special role in steering online networks (Lattemann and Köhler 2005). Young researchers need to develop e-strategies to use research portals to ensure and facilitate authentic human sources for knowledge transfer. While the majority of them have adopted web 2.0 tools already, their willingness to shift from offline to online digital research practices is crucial (Pscheida et al. 2013; 2014) to build trust and protect scientific work in a virtual environment (Lam 2011).

H4::

Novice researchers are more likely to collaborate and work with each other when they receive incentives (as external motivation) that are protected and combined with their trust and the value of their work (as internal motivation).

H4-0::

Incentives as an external motivation can influence ethics as an internal motivation for enhancing research collaboration.

H4-1::

Research ethics as an internal motivation is closely related to research collaboration

3 Method

For this paper, data was collected and analysed through the combination of two main methods: (a) description of a quantitative online survey conducted in the German Federal State of Saxony from 22 July 2012 until 22 October 2012, at the Technische Universität Dresden and (b) forming and testing the structured model. The main aim was to investigate novice researchers’ intentionality to collaborate with each other through the use of web 2.0 and digital online technologies in academia. Our survey included two main parts: the first part reveals demographic data and the second part includes a 5-interval Likert scale with points ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The survey addressed doctoral students as novice researchers who are using web 2.0 technology to communicate and collaborate in research daily life. This 45-item measure was created for this study to assess participants’ perceptions, profiling the nine main factors that shape the final structure of the fish model: task, time, activity, belief, support, context, incentive, ethics, and collaboration. The instrument was then tested by three independent experts in research collaboration before being given to respondents from the target audience. The authors received a total return of n = 140 doctoral students who completed the survey. The data was examined using factor analysis and our fish model was tested with the Partial Least Squares (PLS) technique. SmartPLS, Version 2.0 M3 software was used to test the model (Ringle et al. 2005, p. 1).

4 Results

The majority of respondents (57.71%) were male, 66.74% were not married and had no children, and 30.45% of respondents were from the School of Science, which includes the 13.41% which were PhD students from the Faculty of Mechanical Engineering. This can be considered typical for Saxony’s higher education landscape, as it has a special focus on technical subjects.

4.1 The Measurement Model

PLS is “the second-generation structural equation modelling technique that assesses both the measurement and structural model in a single run” and was chosen for two reasons: it works well for smaller sample sizes and eliminates restrictions on data distribution such as normality (Serenko 2008, p. 465). Before analysing this model, its reliability was measured. Cronbach’s alpha exceeded the required threshold of 0.7 for all items, implying high internal consistency of the scales (Serenko 2008).

In order to submit an accepted level of eligibility for the questionnaire, half of the items (24 of 45 items) were removed which do not have sufficient weight vis-à-vis their main factor (Table 5, see Appendix). Once these items were removed, the model was re-estimated. Reliability results are given in Table 1. The data shows that the measures are robust in terms of their internal composite reliability. The composite reliability of the different items ranges from 0.8 to 1.0, above the recommended starting value of 0.70 (Serenko 2008). In addition, consistent with the guidelines of Fornell and Larcker (Birnholtz 2005), the average variance extracted (AVE) for every component is above 0.50. Table 2 presents the results of measuring the discriminant validity for variable constructs. The matrix diagonal reports that the square roots of the AVEs are greater in all cases than the off-diagonal element in their corresponding row and column, which supports the discriminant validity of the instrument.

Table 1 Assessment of the measurement model
Table 2 Discriminant validity (inter-correlations) of variable constructs

The instrument was tested additionally through PLS-Graph and for convergent validity. Table 4 (see Appendix) shows the factor loading of all items to their respective latent constructs. All items loaded on their respective construct from a lower pound of 0.70 to the upper pound of 0.85. In addition, the T-test of outer model loading in the PLS-Graph output was highly significant (p < 0.001) for each factor’s loading on its respective construct. The results confirm the convergent validity as demonstrating a distinct latent construct.

4.2 The Structured Model

Figure 2 presents the results of the structured model with interaction effect. In order to assess the structured model, a bootstrapping technique was applied. The examination of t-values was based on a 1-tail test with statistically significant levels of p < 0.05 (*), p < 0.01 (**), and p < 0.001 (***). Dotted lines highlight the insignificant paths. Structured components were formulated by multiplying the corresponding indicators of the predictor and moderator construct.

Fig. 2
A fish model illustrates the interaction effect of the research collaboration model. Dotted lines between the blocks represent insignificant paths between ethics, time, beliefs, contexts, tasks, activities, support, and incentives.

Structure model (PLS bootstrapping “path coefficient”). *significant at 0.05 level (1.96); **significant at.01 level (2.58); ***significant at 0.001 level (3.29)

For clarity purposes, the outcomes of the structural model in terms of direct effects, bootstrapping, and t-statistics confirmed the majority of the hypotheses, at various significance levels. However, the results show that only two factors in research collaboration are associated significantly (Fig. 2). Specifically, “Academic activities” is very significantly associated with “Researchers’ beliefs” (H2-0 at β = −0.67, p < 0.001 level). In this first path, “Researchers’ beliefs” has a significant relation with “Collaboration” (H2-1 at β = 0.41, p < 0.001 level). In the second path, “Incentives” and “Ethics” contribute significantly to “Collaboration”. Accordingly, (H4-0) confirms a significant relation between “Incentives” and “Ethics” (H4-0 at β = 0.71, p < 0.001 level) along with the relationship (H4-1) between “Ethics” and shaping “Collaboration” (β = 0.06, p < 0.05).

The other two paths of predicting research collaboration are not significant. First, “Technology and support” has a significant relationship with “Context” (H3-0 at β = 0.64, p < 0.05), but, as a second path, the “Context” cannot predict research “Collaboration” (H3-1 at β = 0.00 not significant). Second, academic “Task” connected strongly with the factor “Time” (H1-0 β = −0.70, p < 0.001). On the other hand, the relationship between “Time” and shaping academic “Collaboration” (H1-1 β = −0.00, not significant) was unrelated in the context of shaping academic collaboration (Table 3).

Table 3 Research hypotheses and conclusions

5 Discussion: Conclusion and Limitations

5.1 Conclusions

The results of this study demonstrate the factors that might influence research collaboration among novice researchers in Germany. The study conceptualised and validated the fish model for understanding research collaboration in the digital age, highlighting where the model can be extended. A brief review of the findings raises the question of what drives researchers’ propensity to collaborate using web 2.0 services.

The first collaboration path showed that doing online doctoral research activities might shape beliefs in using web 2.0 technologies for academic purposes and, thus, enhance collaboration. An example is that using social media to connect with like-minded people eventually shapes one’s belief about the importance of web 2.0. Researchers who believe in using such media are more likely to collaborate and more open to empathy.

Overall this study illustrates how the fish model can be applied to an online setting in order to understand how the interaction between academic activities and researchers’ beliefs can influence research collaboration. The results are consistent with the previous mentioned literature as it was discussed by Terrell et al. (2009), being successful can shape a person’s individual beliefs. Engaging in online research activities in order to communicate and collaborate reflects individual beliefs that control the actions that can enhance further collaboration offline, as has been observed in a professional context (Köhler 1997). Researchers’ activities may reveal some of the individual beliefs that back and catalyse collaboration. When researchers engaged in online research activities, their belief in the use of web 2.0 in research increased. Use of web 2.0 services such as social media can also predict productive and conductive research collaboration (Pscheida et al. 2013).

The second collaboration path shows that in keeping a balance between internal “ethics” and external “incentives”, motivation can confirm collaboration. An example is that researchers’ trust in sharing their ideas via web 2.0 services only grows when they benefit from using such technology and, accordingly, it may lead to collaboration. These findings have important implications for the fish model. Internal and external motivations support future research collaboration. We argue that external and internal motivations are closely related; consequently, in academia both types of motivation help researchers become engaged in collaboration. Higher incentives predict higher levels of trust; researchers are more likely to collaborate when they trust the technologies they use. What motivates researchers to enhance collaboration into the web 2.0 sphere depends on the technologies they can trust and use to extend their professional networks. For collaboration among researchers, trust is synonymous with benefit, which is the catalyst for collaboration.

5.2 Limitations

In this study, research collaboration was defined as the use of web 2.0 technologies for communication and daily research routines (reading, searching, writing, etc.). The authors addressed a subset of the concept labelled e-science or science 2.0. They empirically observed doctoral scholars. These PhD students came mainly from the Faculty of Mathematics and School of Science at the Technische Universität Dresden in Germany. These aspects may limit the range and meaning of the findings presented.

Another limiting aspect is that the fish model reported only two significant paths that may predict research collaboration. It would, however, be more informative if measures of the other paths of the fish model (that appeared as non-significant in our study) were measured once again in a different research context with another sample.