Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Models and Modeling in Science Education ((MMSE,volume 12))

Abstract

The chapter discusses the challenges and opportunities of initiating elementary school children into the practice of scientific modeling. Understanding modeling depends on conceiving science as a process of knowledge construction and critique and of pragmatic engagement with material and conditions that enable and mediate scientific ways of knowing. Our 25 years of research with children indicates the necessity of inviting students to invent models to address questions they care about, test their models in contexts that provide feedback about model fit and provoke model revision, and participate in a community engaged in building knowledge together. An accessible entrée to modeling is through developing representational competence. As students begin to appreciate the purposes and trade-offs of various representational conventions, they also begin to address more sophisticated questions about model fit, including whether and how a particular model can be accepted as a valid representation of a target phenomenon.

This material is based on work supported by the National Science Foundation under grant no. DRL-1316312.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Berland, L. K., & Reiser, B. J. (2011). Classroom communities’ adaptations of the practice of scientific argumentation. Science Education, 95(2), 191–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chinn, C. A., & Malhotra, B. A. (2002). Epistemologically authentic inquiry in schools: A theoretical framework for evaluating inquiry tasks. Science Education, 86(2), 175–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J. (2009). The role of imagistic simulation in scientific thought experiments. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 686–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb, P., Confrey, J., diSessa, A., Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2003). Design experiments in education. Educational Researcher, 32(1), 9–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danish, J. A., & Enyedy, N. (2007). Negotiated representational mediators: How young children decide what to include in their science representations. Science Education, 91(1), 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. (2004). Metarepresentation: Naïve competence and targets for instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22(3), 293–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M. (2015). Educational implications of choosing “practice” to describe science in the next generation science standards. Science Education, 99(8), 1041–1048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Toupin, C. (1986). Systematicity and surface similarity in the development of analogy. Cognitive Science, 10(3), 277–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, D. (1990). Experiment and the making of meaning: Human agency in scientific observation and experiment. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gooding, D. C. (2006). From phenomenology to field theory: Faraday’s visual reasoning. Perspectives on Science, 14(1), 40–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, N. (1976). Languages of art. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett Publishing Company, Inc.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Grosslight, L., Unger, C., Jay, E., & Smith, C. L. (1991). Understanding models and their use in science: Conceptions of middle and high school students and experts. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28(9), 799–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hesse, M. B. (1962). Forces and fields. The concept of action at a distance in the history of physics. New York: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hestenes, D. (1992). Modeling games in the Newtonian world. American Journal of Physics, 60(8), 732–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klayman, J., & Ha, Y.-W. (1987). Confirmation, disconfirmation, and information in hypothesis testing. Psychological Review, 94(2), 211–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konold, C., & Kazak, S. (2008). Reconnecting data and chance. Technology Innovations in Statistics Education, 2(1), Center for the Teaching of Statistics, Department of Statistics, UCLA, UC Los Angeles.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1990). Drawing things together. In M. E. Lynch & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice (pp. 19–68). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1999). Pandora’s hope: Essays on the reality of science studies. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., Horvath, J., & Schauble, L. (1994). Developing model-based reasoning. Interactive Learning Environments, 4(3), 218–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2000). The development of model-based reasoning. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 39–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2003). Origins and evolution of model-based reasoning in mathematics and science. In H. Doerr & R. Lesh (Eds.), Beyond constructivism: A models and modeling perspective (pp. 59–70). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2004). Modeling natural variation through distribution. American Educational Research Journal, 41(3), 635–679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2006). Scientific thinking and science literacy. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, K. A. Renninger, & I. E. Sigel (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Child psychology in practice) (Vol. 4, 6th ed., pp. 153–196). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2012). Seeding evolutionary thinking by engaging children in modeling its foundations. Science Education, 96(4), 701–724.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2013). Representational re-description as a catalyst of conceptual change. In B. M. Brizuela & B. E. Gravel (Eds.), “Show me what you know”: Exploring student representations across STEM disciplines (pp. 244–249). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2015). Developing scientific reasoning: The role of epistemic practices. In R. Lerner, L. S. Liben, & U. Mueller (Eds.), Child psychology and developmental science (Cognitive processes) (Vol. 2, 7th ed., pp. 671–714). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2017). Children’s conception of sampling in local ecosystem investigations. Science Education, 101(6), 968–985.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., Schauble, L., Carpenter, S., & Penner, D. (2000). The inter-related development of inscriptions and conceptual understanding. In P. Cobb, E. Yackel, & K. McClain (Eds.), Symbolizing and communicating in mathematics classrooms: Perspectives on discourse, tools, and instructional design (pp. 325–360). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, R., Schauble, L., & Lucas, D. (2008). Supporting development of the epistemology of inquiry. Cognitive Development, 23(4), 512–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, D., Broderick, N., Lehrer, R., & Bohanan, R. (2005). Making the grounds of scientific inquiry visible in the classroom. Science Scope, 29(3), 39–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manz, E. (2015). Resistance and the development of scientific practice: Designing ‘the Mangle’ into science instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 33(2), 89–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metz, K. (1998). Emergent understanding and attribution of randomness: Comparative analysis of the reasoning of primary grade children and undergraduates. Cognition and Instruction, 16(3), 285–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metz, K. (2004). Children’s understanding of scientific inquiry: Their conceptualization of uncertainty in investigations of their own design. Cognition and Instruction, 22(2), 219–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (2008). Model-based reasoning in scientific practice. In R. A. Duschl & R. E. Grandy (Eds.), Teaching scientific inquiry: Recommendations for research and implementation (pp. 57–79). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J., & Chandrasekharan, S. (2009). Hybrid analogies in conceptual innovation in science. Cognitive Systems Research Journal, Special Issue: Integrating Cognitive Abilities, 10, 178–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • NGSS Lead States. (2013). Next generation science standards: For states, by states. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penner, D., Giles, N., Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (1997). Building functional models: Designing an elbow. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 34(2), 125–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, A. (1995). The mangle of practice: Time, agency, and science. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rouse, J. (2007). Practice theory. Division I Faculty Publications (Paper 43). http://wesscholar.wesleyan.edu/div1facpubs/43

  • Sandoval, W. (2004). Developing learning theories by refining conjectures embodied in educational designs. Educational Psychologist, 39(4), 213–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandoval, W. (2014). Science education’s need for a theory of epistemological development. Science Education, 98(3), 383–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schauble, L. (1990). Belief revision in children: The role of prior knowledge and strategies for generating evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 49(1), 31–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapin, S., & Schaffer, S. (1985). Leviathan and the air pump: Hobbes, Boyle, and the experimental life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tytler, R., Prain, V., Hubber, R., & Waldrip, B. (2013). Constructing representations to learn in science. Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vertesi, J. (2014). Drawing as: Distinctions and disambiguation in digital images of Mars. In C. Coopmans, J. Vertesi, M. Lynch, & S. Woolgar (Eds.), Representation in scientific practice revisited (pp. 15–35). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Richard Lehrer or Leona Schauble .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Lehrer, R., Schauble, L. (2019). Learning to Play the Modeling Game. In: Upmeier zu Belzen, A., Krüger, D., van Driel, J. (eds) Towards a Competence-Based View on Models and Modeling in Science Education. Models and Modeling in Science Education, vol 12. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-30255-9_13

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-30255-9_13

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-30254-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-30255-9

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics