Skip to main content

Atypical Interaction: An Introduction

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Atypical Interaction

Abstract

Wilkinson, Rae and Rasmussen provide a thorough, state-of-the-art Introduction to the growing field of Atypical Interaction, that is, conversations and other types of social interaction where one of the participants has a communicative impairment or communication disorder. These can include, for example, autism, dementia, learning disability, stammering or hearing impairment. The authors discuss how within this field the method of conversation analysis is used to record, transcribe and analyse these types of social interaction. They describe similarities and differences in the way that the different forms of communicative impairment can impact on social interaction, and they summarise the contribution that work in this areas has made to both communication disorder research and conversation analysis research on the nature of talk and social interaction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A note on terminology. The field of communicative impairments/communication disorders is one which has been the focus of attention from a number of different disciplines, with medicine (including psychiatry), psychology (including neuropsychology, clinical psychology and developmental psychology), linguistics, and speech pathology and therapy being among those which have had most influence on the field. While social science perspectives, such as that in the current volume, have perhaps been less in evidence historically, this has changed somewhat in recent years, with, for example, the social model of disability (Shakespeare 1998) becoming influential. Overall, therefore, there is a wide range of perspectives and terminology within the field. The approach in this volume is primarily a descriptive one, aiming to capture aspects of the talk and other interactional conduct of the participants in these interactions, while at the same time retaining an awareness that at least one of the participants has a particular condition or conditions which are impacting on their talk and/or conduct. In the vast majority of cases, these conditions will have been assessed or diagnosed by a medical or other professional. The interactions here are ‘atypical’ in that they display differences in systematic ways to the practices that have been described by conversation analysts in relation to ‘typical’ interaction (i.e. here, in people without communicative impairments). We use the term ‘impairment’ to refer to aspects of the condition which impact on talk and interaction. These can be, for example, linguistic, cognitive, motor or sensory in nature and are often only evident in mundane interaction through their impact on talk or conduct. The term is not used here in the sense of the talk/conduct being ‘impaired’. To take one example: the use of sign language is ‘atypical’ in that its use of the visual-manual channel as the primary mode of communication makes it different from spoken talk, the most common form of human face-to-face communication, which uses the auditory-vocal channel. While sign language is by no means an ‘impaired’ form of communication, it can be used when the person has a hearing impairment (see Girard-Groeber, this volume).

  2. 2.

    This is not to deny the risk of the interaction being affected in some manner by the participants’ awareness of being recorded i.e. the ‘observer’s paradox’ (Labov 1972). For some ways in which this type of research attempts to overcome, or at least minimize, these risks see Goodwin (1993).

  3. 3.

    For further details of the development of CA, see Schegloff (1992a).

  4. 4.

    The term ‘talk-in-interaction’ is used to refer to talk within social interaction generally, thus including both conversation and institutional interaction (Schegloff 2007).

  5. 5.

    For an outline of conversation analytic work on ‘preference’ see Pomerantz and Heritage (2013).

  6. 6.

    A related strand of ethnomethodologically-inspired work on communicative impairments and disability more generally should also be noted here. See, for example, work on interactions between children with severe learning disabilities and their family members (e.g. Pollner and McDonald-Wikler 1985; Goode 1994), and Robillard (1999) on the lived experience of motor neurone disease. For reflections on ethnomethodological studies of disability, see Goode (2003).

  7. 7.

    While this difficulty with pragmatics appears to be a central feature of the communication problems of people with these conditions, in many cases other aspects of language (such as lexis and grammar) or speech may also be affected.

  8. 8.

    For other CA investigations which examine particular interactional practices in both aphasia and dysarthria see Bloch and Beeke (2008) and Wilkinson et al. (2011).

  9. 9.

    It is important to keep in mind that not every trouble source in the talk of people with communicative impairments will necessarily be linked to those impairments (cf. Schegloff 2003). As is evident in the talk of typical speakers, repair can be initiated for a number of reasons, not all of which are to do with errors or infelicities in talk (Schegloff et al. 1977).

  10. 10.

    The talk of speakers with non-fluent aphasia (Kent 2004) can also regularly be treated as a trouble through an interlocutor producing an other-initiation of repair (e.g. Heeschen and Schegloff 1999).

  11. 11.

    It appears that these ways of talking and conducting oneself are not always consciously adopted by the participants (Heeschen and Schegloff 1999).

References

  • Abbeduto, L., & Rosenberg, S. (1980). The communicative competence of mildly retarded adults. Applied Psycholinguistics,1(4), 405–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Acton, C. (2004). A conversation analytic perspective on stammering: Some reflections and observations. Stammering Research,1(3), 249–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Angeleri, R., Bosco, F. M., Zettin, M., Sacco, K., Colle, L., & Bara, B. G. (2008). Communicative impairment in traumatic brain injury: A complete pragmatic assessment. Brain and Language,107(3), 229–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antaki, C. (2011). Six kinds of applied conversation analysis. In C. Antaki (Ed.), Applied conversation analysis: Intervention and change in institutional talk (pp. 1–14). Houndmills, Basingstoke and New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, S. (2005). Co-constructing meaning in dysarthria: Word and letter repetition in the construction of turns. In K. Richards & P. Seedhouse (Eds.), Applying conversation analysis (pp. 38–55). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, S., & Beeke, S. (2008). Co-constructed talk in the conversations of people with dysarthria and aphasia. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,22(12), 974–990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, S., & Wilkinson, R. (2009). Acquired dysarthria in conversation: Identifying sources of understanding problems. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,44(5), 769–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Button, G., & Casey, N. (1984). Generating topic: The use of topic initial elicitors. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis (pp. 167–190). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clift, R. (2016). Conversation analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, S., Markova, I., & Murphy, J. (1997). Bringing conversations to a close: The management of closings in interactions between AAC users and ‘natural’ speakers. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,11(6), 467–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denman, A., & Wilkinson, R. (2011). Applying conversation analysis to traumatic brain injury: Investigating touching another person in everyday social interaction. Disability and Rehabilitation,33(3), 243–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dingemanse, M., Roberts, S. G., Baranova, J., Blythe, J., Drew, P., Floyd, S., et al. (2015). Universal principles in the repair of communication problems. PLoS One,10(9), e0136100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dobbinson, S., Perkins, M., & Boucher, J. (2003). The interactional significance of formulas in autistic language. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,17(4–5), 299–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drew, P. (1997). ‘Open’ class repair initiators in response to sequential sources of trouble in conversation. Journal of Pragmatics,28, 69–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drew, P., & Heritage, J. (1992). Analyzing talk at work: An introduction. In P. Drew & J. Heritage (Eds.), Talk at work (pp. 3–65). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drew, P., & Penn, C. (2016). On failure to understand what the other is saying: Accountability, incongruity, and miscommunication. In J. D. Robinson (Ed.), Accountability in social interaction (pp. 47–72). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Egbert, M., & Deppermann, A. (Eds.). (2012). Hearing aids communication: Integrating social interaction, audiology and user centered design to improve communication with hearing loss and hearing technologies. Mannheim: Verlag für Gesprächsforschung.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eggins, S. (1994). An introduction to systemic functional linguistics. London: Pinter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freed, D. (2000). Motor speech disorders: Diagnosis & treatment. San Diego, CA: Singular.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, R., & Wagner, J. (Eds.). (2005). Second language conversations. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (1963). A conception and experiments with “trust” as a condition of stable concerted actions. In O. J. Harvey (Ed.), Motivation and social interaction (pp. 187–238). New York: Ronald Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1964). The neglected situation. American Anthropologist,66, 133–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1979). Footing. Semiotica, 25(1/2), 1–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goode, D. (1994). A world without words. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goode, D. (2003). Ethnomethodology and disability studies: A Reflection on Robillard. Human Studies,26(4), 493–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., & Barresi, B. (2001). The Boston diagnostic aphasia examination (BDAE) (3rd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1993). Recording human interaction in natural settings. Pragmatics: Quarterly Publication of the International Pragmatics Association (IPrA),3(2), 181–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1995). Co-constructing meaning in conversations with an aphasic man. Research on Language and Social Interaction,28, 233–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2000). Gesture, aphasia and interaction. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture: Window into thought and action (pp. 84–98). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (Ed.). (2003). Conversation and brain damage. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the ways of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guitar, B. (2013). Stuttering: An integrated approach to its nature and treatment (4th ed.). Baltimore: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heeschen, C., & Schegloff, E. A. (1999). Agrammatism, adaptation theory, conversation analysis: On the role of so-called telegraphic style in talk-in-interaction. Aphasiology,13(4/5), 365–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heeschen, C., & Schegloff, E. A. (2003). Aphasic agrammatism as interactional artifact and achievement. In C. Goodwin (Ed.), Conversation and brain damage. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helasvuo, M.-L., Laakso, M., & Sorjonen, M.-L. (2004). Searching for words: Syntactic and sequential construction of word search in conversations of Finnish speakers with aphasia. Research in Language and Social Interaction,37(1), 1–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilari, K., Byng, S., Lamping, D. L., & Smith, S. C. (2003). Stroke and aphasia quality of life scale-39 (SAQOL-39) evaluation of acceptability, reliability, and validity. Stroke,34(8), 1944–1950.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holland, A. L. (1982). Observing functional communication of aphasic adults. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,47(1), 50–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferson, G. (2004). Glossary of transcript symbols with an introduction. In G. Lerner (Ed.), Conversation analysis: Studies from the first generation (pp. 13–31). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jingree, T., Finlay, W. M. L., & Antaki, C. (2006). Empowering words, disempowering actions: An analysis of interactions between staff members and people with learning disabilities in residents’ meetings. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,50(3), 212–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, D. (2015). A family living with Alzheimer’s disease: The communicative challenges. Dementia,14(5), 555–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kent, R. D. (2004). The MIT encyclopedia of communication disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidwell, M. (2013). Interaction among children. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 511–532). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kindell, J., Sage, K., Keady, J., & Wilkinson, R. (2013). Adapting to conversation with semantic dementia: Using enactment as a compensatory strategy in everyday social interaction. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,48(5), 497–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitzinger, C., & Jones, D. (2007). When may calls home: The opening moments of family telephone conversations with an Alzheimer’s patient. Feminism & Psychology,17(2), 184–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Korkiakangas, T., Dindar, K., Laitila, A., & Kärnä, E. (2016). The Sally-Anne test: An interactional analysis of a dyadic assessment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,51(6), 685–702.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. (1972). Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, L. C., Harrington, R. A., Louie, B. B., & Newschaffer, C. J. (2008). Children with autism: Quality of life and parental concerns. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,38(6), 1147–1160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, G. H. (1996). On the “semi-permeable” character of grammatical units in conversation: Conditional entry into the turn space of another speaker. In E. Ochs, E. Schegloff, & S. Thompson (Eds.), Interaction and grammar (pp. 238–276). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lind, M., & Sønsterud, H. (2014). Co-construction of turns at talk: Active listening or disruptions in conversation with persons who stutter? Journal of Interactional Research in Communication Disorders/Equinox,5(2), 141–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindholm, C. (2015). Parallel realities: The interactional management of confabulation in dementia care encounters. Research in Language and Social Interaction,48(2), 176–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Local, J., & Wootton, T. (1995). Interactional and phonetic aspects of immediate echolalia in autism: A case study. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,9(2), 155–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule–2nd edition (ADOS-2). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubinski, R., Duchan, D., & Weitzner-Lin, B. (1980). Analysis of breakdowns and repairs in adult aphasic conversation. In R. Brookshire (Ed.), Clinical aphasiology conference proceedings (pp. 111–116). Minneapolis: BRK Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mates, A. W., Mikesell, L., & Smith, M. S. (2010). Language, interaction, and frontotemporal dementia: Reverse engineering the social mind. London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard, D. W., & Turowetz, J. (2017). Doing diagnosis: Autism, interaction order, and the use of narrative in clinical talk. Social Psychology Quarterly,80(3), 254–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, R., Leudar, I., & Antaki, C. (2004). Do people with schizophrenia display theory of mind deficits in clinical interactions? Psychological Medicine, 34(3), 401–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, R., Skelton, J., Heath, C., Burns, T., & Priebe, S. (2002). Engagement of patients with psychosis in the consultation: Conversation analytic study [with commentary by J Skelton]. BMJ,325(7373), 1148–1151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCleary, L., & de Arantes Leite, T. (2013). Turn-taking in Brazilian sign language: Evidence from overlap. Journal of Interactional Research in Communication Disorders,4(1), 123–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McIlvenny, P. (1995). Seeing conversations: Analyzing sign language talk. In P. ten Have & G. Psathas (Eds.), Situated order: Studies in the social organization of talk and embodied activities (pp. 129–150). Washington, DC: University Press of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merrison, S., & Merrison, A. J. (2005). Repair in speech and language therapy interaction: Investigating pragmatic language impairment in children. Child Language Teaching and Therapy,21, 191–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mikesell, L. (2009). Conversational practices of a frontotemporal dementia patient and his interlocutors. Research on Language & Social Interaction,42, 135–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muskett, T., Perkins, M., Clegg, J., & Body, R. (2010). Inflexibility as an interactional phenomenon: Using conversation analysis to re-examine a symptom of autism. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,24(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norén, N., Samuelsson, C., & Plejert, C. (Eds.). (2013). Aided communication in everyday interaction. London: J&R Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pajo, K. (2013). The occurrence of ‘what’, ‘where’, ‘what house’ and other repair initiations in the home environment of hearing-impaired individuals. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders,48(1), 66–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollner, M., & McDonald-Wikler, L. (1985). The social construction of unreality: A case study of a family’s attribution of competence to a severely retarded child. Family Process,24(2), 241–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, A., & Heritage, J. (2013). Preference. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 210–228). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price-Williams, D., & Sabsay, S. (1979). Communicative competence among severely retarded persons. Semiotica,26(1–2), 35–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapley, M. (2004). The social construction of intellectual disability. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rapley, M., & Antaki, C. (1996). A conversation analysis of the ‘acquiescence’ of people with learning disabilities. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology,6(3), 207–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, G. (2013). That’s my story! Resisting disabling processes in a therapeutic activity. Journal of Interactional Research in Communication Disorders,4(2), 273–298. https://doi.org/10.1558/jircd.v4i2.273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, G. (2017). The use of pictures for interactional purposes and the grammar of social interaction. Journal of Interactional Research in Communication Disorders,18(1), 121–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, G. (2018). On the social constraints of having a world. RASK: International Journal of Language and Communication, 7, 79–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rasmussen, G., Andersen, E. M., & Kristiansen, E. D. (2019). Working out availability, unavailability, and awayness in social face-to-face encounters—The case of dementia. Discourse Studies,21(3), 258–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robillard, A. B. (1999). Meaning of disability: The lived experience of paralysis. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language,50(4), 696–735. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1974.0010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samuelsson, C. (2009). Using conversation analysis to study prosodic problems in a child with language impairment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy,25, 59–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarno, M. T. (1998). Acquired aphasia (3rd ed.). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sataloff, J., & Sataloff, R. T. (Eds.). (2005). Hearing loss (4th ed.). New York: Taylor and Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. (1979). The relevance of repair to syntax-for-conversation. In T. Givón (Ed.), Syntax and semantics (Vol. 12, pp. 261–286). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1992a). Introduction. In H. Sacks, Lectures on conversation (G. Jefferson, Ed.). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1992b). Repair after next turn: The last structurally provided defense of intersubjectivity in conversation. American Journal of Sociology,97, 1295–1345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (1996). Turn organization: One intersection of grammar and interaction. In E. Ochs, E. A. Schegloff, & S. A. Thompson (Eds.), Interaction and grammar (pp. 52–133). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (2003). Conversation analysis and communication disorders. In C. Goodwin (Ed.), Conversation and brain damage (pp. 21–55). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (2006). Interaction: The infrastructure for social institutions, the natural ecological niche for language, and the arena in which culture is enacted. In N. J. Enfield & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction (pp. 70–96). London: Berg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. (2007). Sequence organization in interaction: A Primer in conversation analysis (Vol. 1). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E., Jefferson, G., & Sacks, H. (1977). The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair for conversation. Language,53, 361–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E., & Sacks, H. (1973). Opening up closings. Semiotica,8(4), 289–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schienberg, S., & Holland, A. (1980). Conversational turn taking in Wernicke’s aphasia. In R. Brookshire (Ed.), Clinical aphasiology conference proceedings (pp. 106–110). Minneapolis: BRK Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shakespeare, T. (1998). The disability reader: Social science perspectives. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, O., Heritage, J., Yin, L., Maynard, D. W., & Bauman, M. L. (2016). ‘What brings him here today?’: Medical problem presentation involving children with autism spectrum disorders and typically developing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,46(2), 378–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • St. Clair, M. C., Pickles, A., Durkin, K., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2011). A longitudinal study of behavioral, emotional and social difficulties in individuals with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Communication Disorders,44(2), 186–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterponi, L., de Kirby, K., & Shankey, J. (2015). Rethinking language in autism. Autism,19(5), 517–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stopford, C. L., Thompson, J. C., Neary, D., Richardson, A. M., & Snowden, J. S. (2012). Working memory, attention, and executive function in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Cortex,48(4), 429–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stribling, P., & Rae, J. (2010). Interactional analysis of scaffolding in a mathematical task in ASD. In H. Gardner & M. Forrester (Eds.), Analysing interactions in childhood. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stribling, P., Rae, J., & Dickerson, P. (2007). Two forms of spoken repetition in a girl with autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders,42(4), 427–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tarplee, C., & Barrow, E. (1999). Delayed echoing as an interactional resource: A case study of a 3-year-old child on the autistic spectrum. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics,13(6), 449–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetnowski, J. A., & Damico, J. S. (2001). A demonstration of the advantages of qualitative methodologies in stuttering research. Journal of Fluency Disorders,26, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, L., Howes, C., & McCabe, R. (2016). Effect of questions used by psychiatrists on therapeutic alliance and adherence. The British Journal of Psychiatry,209(1), 40–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Togher, L., & Hand, L. (1998). Use of politeness markers with different communication partners: An investigation of five subjects with traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology,12(7–8), 755–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tykkyläinen, T. (2010). Child-initiated repair in task interactions. In H. Gardner & M. Forrester (Eds.), Analysing interactions in childhood: Insights from conversation analysis (pp. 227–248). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, R. (2014). Intervening with conversation analysis in speech and language therapy: Improving aphasic conversation. Research on Language and Social Interaction,47(3), 219–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, R. (2019). Atypical interaction: Conversation analysis and communicative impairments. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 52(3), 281–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, R., Beeke, S., & Maxim, J. (2003). Adapting to conversation: On the use of pro-forms by aphasic speakers in the construction of turns at talk. In C. Goodwin (Ed.), Conversation and brain damage (pp. 59–89). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, R., Beeke, S., & Maxim, J. (2010). Formulating actions and events with limited linguistic resources: Enactment and iconicity in agrammatic aphasic talk. Research on Language and Social Interaction,43(1), 57–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, R., Bloch, S., & Clarke, M. (2011). On the use of graphic resources in interaction by people with communication disorders. In J. Streeck, C. Goodwin, & C. LeBaron (Eds.), Embodied interaction: Language and body in the material world (pp. 152–168). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, V., Ponting, L., Ford, K., Rudge, P., & (Skills for Support Team). (2010). Skills for support: Personal assistants and people with learning disabilities. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(1), 59–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wootton, A. J. (1999). An investigation of delayed echoing in a child with autism. First Language,19(57), 359–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yearley, S., & Brewer, J. D. (1989). Stigma and conversational competence: A conversation analytic study of the mentally handicapped. Human Studies,12, 97–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Wilkinson, R., Rae, J.P., Rasmussen, G. (2020). Atypical Interaction: An Introduction. In: Wilkinson, R., Rae, J.P., Rasmussen, G. (eds) Atypical Interaction. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-28799-3_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics