Skip to main content

Updating Dewey’s Transactional Theory of Action in Connection with Evolutionary Theory

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
John Dewey and the Notion of Trans-action

Part of the book series: Palgrave Studies in Relational Sociology ((PSRS))

  • 499 Accesses

Abstract

John Dewey’s ‘habit’-centred, transactional theory of action gets rid of the Cartesian mind–world dualism by replacing ‘mind-first’ explanations of action with ‘action-first’ explanations of the mind. Dewey embraced Darwinian thinking, and built his theory of action in an evolutionary framework. Of course, he could not have foreseen all the developments in theory and research on human evolution that have unfolded since his time; evolutionary theory and understanding have been elaborated, for example, with the idea of co-evolutionary niche construction. This chapter outlines an update of Dewey’s transactional theory of action, drawing on, among others, Daniel Dennett’s, Andy Clark’s, and Kim Sterelny’s works, and some recent developments in theories and research on the brain, mind, and the evolution of human culture. A sort of ‘four E’—extensive, enactive, embodied, and embedded—notion of the mind is considered, as well as other recent ideas and conceptual tools, such as (socio-cultural) niche construction, the Sterelnian apprentice learning setup, the Clarkian conception of predictive processing as a central function of the brain, affordances of action, and the Dennettian understanding that in evolution, competence comes before comprehension, and the latter could arise only with culture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    In this chapter, consistently with a long line of our previous works going back for decades, we write the term ‘transaction’ and its derivatives without the hyphen.

  2. 2.

    The switch from the ‘mind-first’ to the ‘action-first’ explanation, to put it briefly, is the idea that we should not seek to explain action with anything in the mind—beliefs and desires, or volition—but instead, conceive action, channelled by habits, as the baseline of all life that also explains whatever content we might want to say there is to someone’s mind (see, e.g., Joas and Kilpinen 2006).

  3. 3.

    In fact, pragmatism was unpopular in those days, in Europe in particular. An important change started in America in the 1970s, with Putnam’s (1975) and Rorty’s (1979) neo-pragmatism overcoming the subject–object dualism by emphasizing the importance of language for the human mind and the Wittgensteinian and Deweyan insight that there is no private language, that linguistic ‘“meanings” just ain’t in the head’ (Putnam 1975, p. 144).

  4. 4.

    It is akin to certain much older ideas, though, for instance, those that go under the name of ‘Baldwin effect’ (after James Mark Baldwin), which somewhat similarly emphasized the role of behavioural plasticity and learning in evolution, and took into account ‘the social aspect of evolution’—‘social relations and traditions’ in the evolving population—as an important part of the environment of selection (Richards 1987, p. 484; see Weber and Depew 2003).

  5. 5.

    ‘The organism acts in accordance with its own structure, simple or complex, upon its surroundings,’ Dewey (1988a [1922]) said, and stressed that ‘[a]s a consequence the changes produced in the environment react upon the organism and its activities. The living creature undergoes, suffers, the consequences of its own behavior’ (129). ‘The higher’ (that is to say, the more neurologically complex and phenotypically flexible) ‘the form of life,’ he also knew, ‘the more important is the active reconstruction of the medium’ (128). ‘Of human organisms it is especially true that activities carried on for satisfying needs so change the environment that new needs arise which demand still further change in the activities of the organism by which they are satisfied; and so on in potentially endless chain’ (Dewey 1991, p. 35).

  6. 6.

    This may be said of any life form. The most well-worn example of animal niche construction is probably beaver dams, but even relatively simple animals (spiders are a good example) utilize tools, such as traps to catch prey, and many more build nests, or store food; even plants and bacteria may be said to be engaged in some niche construction, because in their transactions with the environment, they tend to change the composition of their surroundings in many ways (see Odling-Smee et al. 2003; Sterelny 2005).

  7. 7.

    Ecological inheritance intertwined with (the evolution of) genetic and cultural inheritance is also referred to as ‘triple-inheritance’ (Laland and O’Brien 2010, p. 312; Odling-Smee and Laland 2011, p. 223).

  8. 8.

    As Hutto (2018; Hutto and Myin 2017, pp. 82–85, see 2013, Ch. 7) argues, Clark still clings to an unfortunate cognitivist framework where (the majority of) cognitive processing is thought to take place inside the body (only sometimes ‘extending’ partially out of it). Most cognitive scientists have been talking in terms of representations, and thus, arguably unnecessarily keeping the door open for ‘creeping Cartesianism’ which pragmatists like Dewey expelled (Solymosi 2013, p. 594).

  9. 9.

    Dennett is indifferent to the terminological novelty of niche construction because he is well aware of Baldwinian and related ideas having been around for more than a century under different names, and thinks that they were incorporated even in mainstream evolutionary synthesis in the 1980s (see Dennett 2004, pp. 725–726).

  10. 10.

    Of course, this is well in keeping with the Dewey’s action-first vein of explanation.

References

  • Allis, C. D., Caparros, M.-L., Jenuwein, T., & Reinberg, D. (Eds.). (2015). Epigenetics (2nd ed.). Cold Spring Harbor: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.). (1992). The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (2009). Adam’s Tongue: How Humans Made Language, How Language Made Humans. New York: Hill and Wang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P., & Wacquant, L. (1992). An Invitation to Reflexive Sociology. Cambridge: Polity.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., Richerson, P., & Henrich, J. (2011). The Cultural Niche: Why Social Learning Is Essential for Human Adaptation. PNAS, 108(suppl. 2), 10918–10925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buller, D. (2005). Adapting Minds: Evolutionary Psychology and the Persistent Quest for Human Nature. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary Psychology: A New Paradigm for Psychological Science. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, A. (2009). Radical Embodied Cognitive Science. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2002). On Nature and Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2008). Biolinguistic Explorations: Design, Development, Evolution. International Journal of Philosophical Studies, 15, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2005). Book Review of Kim Sterelny’s Thought in a Hostile World. Mind, 114, 777–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action and Cognitive Extension. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2015). Predicting Peace: An End to the Representation Wars. In T. K. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND (Vol. 7). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2016). Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action and the Embodied Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The Extended Mind. Analysis, 58, 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (2006 [1976]). The Selfish Gene (30th Anniversary ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Human Brain. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. (2003). Multilevel Selection in a Complex Adaptive System: The Problem of Language Origins. In B. H. Weber & D. J. Depew (Eds.), Evolution and Learning: The Baldwin Effect Reconsidered (pp. 81–106). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1987). The Intentional Stance. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness Explained. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s Dangerous Idea: Evolution and the Meanings of Life. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (2003). The Baldwin Effect: A Crane, Not a Skyhook. In B. H. Weber & D. J. Depew (Eds.), Evolution and Learning: The Baldwin Effect Reconsidered (pp. 69–79). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (2004). An Evolutionary Perspective on Cognition: Through a Glass Lightly. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 35, 721–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (2017). From Bacteria to Bach and Back: The Evolution of Minds. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1896). The Reflex Arc Concept in Psychology. Psychological Review, 3, 357–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1977 [1909]). The Influence of Darwinism on Philosophy. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), The Middle Works of John Dewey (Vol. 4, pp. 3–14). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1983 [1922]). Human Nature and Conduct: The Middle Works of John Dewey (Vol. 14, J. A. Boydston, Ed.). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1985 [1916]). Democracy and Education: The Middle Works of John Dewey (Vol. 9, J. A. Boydston, Ed.). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1988a [1920]). Reconstruction in Philosophy. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), The Middle Works of John Dewey (Vol. 12, pp. 77–201). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1988b [1925]). Experience and Nature: The Later Works of John Dewey (Vol. 1, J. A. Boydston, Ed.). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1988c [1929]). The Quest for Certainty. The Later Works of John Dewey (Vol. 4, J. A. Boydston, Ed.). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1991 [1938]). Logic. The Theory of Inquiry. The Later Works of John Dewey (Vol. 12, J. A. Boydston, Ed.). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J., & Bentley, A. (1991 [1949]). Knowing and the Known. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.), The Later Works of John Dewey (Vol. 16, pp. 1–294). Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (2001). A Mind So Rare: The Evolution of Human Consciousness. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. (1996). Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. London: Faber and Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durham, W. H. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, Cultures, and Human Diversity. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleetwood, S. (2008). Structure, Institution, Agency, Habit, and Reflexive Deliberation. Journal of Institutional Economics, 4, 183–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2008). Intersubjectivity in Perception. Continental Philosophy Review, 41, 163–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2017). Enactivist Interventions: Rethinking the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. (1986). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2005). The Social Construction of the Cultural Mind: Imitative Learning as a Mechanism of Human Pedagogy. Interaction Studies, 6, 463–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, B. K. (1992). Evolutionary Developmental Biology. New York: Chapman & Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haugeland, J. (1995). Mind Embodied and Embedded. In L. Haaparanta & S. Heinämaa (Eds.), Mind and Cognition: Philosophical Perspectives on Cognitive Science and Artificial Intelligence (pp. 209–237). Helsinki: Societas Philosophica Fennica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The Faculty of Language: What Is It, Who Has It, and How Did It Evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2017). The Evolution of Our Species: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making Us Smarter. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heras-Escribano, M., & De Pinedo-Garcia, M. (2018). Affordances and Landscapes: Overcoming the Nature-Culture Dichotomy Through Niche Construction Theory. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2294. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2012). New Thinking: The Evolution of Human Cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 2091–2096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2016). Born Pupils? Natural Pedagogy and Cultural Pedagogy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 280–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2018). Cognitive Gadgets: The Cultural Evolution of Thinking. Cambridge: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgson, G. M. (2004). The Evolution of Institutional Economics: Agency, Structure and Darwinism in American Institutionalism. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Holliday, R. (2006). Epigenetics: A Historical Overview. Epigenetics, 1, 76–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, S. (1998). Vehicles, Contents, Conceptual Structure, and Externalism. Analysis, 58(1), 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. S. (2018). Getting into Predictive Processing’s Great Guessing Game: Bootstrap Heaven or Hell? Synthese, 195(6), 2445–2458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D., Kirchhoff, M. D., & Myin, E. (2014). Extensive Enactivism: Why Keep It All in? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00706.

  • Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing Enactivism: Basic Minds Without Content. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2017). Evolving Enactivism: Basic Minds Meet Content. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Joas, H. (1996). The Creativity of Action. Cambridge: Polity.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joas, H., & Kilpinen, E. (2006). Creativity and Society. In R. Shook & J. Margolis (Eds.), A Companion to Pragmatism. Blackwell Companions to Philosophy (pp. 323–335). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, E. F. (2000). The Century of the Gene. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, E. F. (2005). The Century Beyond the Gene. Journal of Biosciences, 30, 3–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendal, J., Tehrani, J. J., & Odling-Smee, J. (2011). Introduction: Human Niche Construction in Interdisciplinary Focus. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 785–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilpinen, E. (2000). The Enormous Fly-Wheel of Society: Pragmatism’s Habitual Conception of Action and Social Theory. Helsinki: University of Helsinki.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., & Piiroinen, T. (2004). The Relevance of Ontological Commitments in Social Sciences: Realist and Pragmatist Viewpoints. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 34, 231–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., & Piiroinen, T. (2006). Toward Pragmatist Methodological Relationalism: From Philosophizing Sociology to Sociologizing Philosophy. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 36, 303–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., & Piiroinen, T. (2007). Socializing Metaphysics and Mind: A Pragmatist Point of View on the Methodology of the Social Sciences. Human Studies, 39, 97–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., & Piiroinen, T. (2012). On the Distinctively Human: Two Perspectives on the Evolution of Language and Conscious Mind. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 42, 87–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., & Piiroinen, T. (2013). Human Transaction Mechanisms in Evolutionary Niches—A Methodological Relationalist Standpoint. In F. Dépelteau & C. Powell (Eds.), Applying Relational Sociology: Relations, Networks, and Society (pp. 83–100). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., & Piiroinen, T. (2018a). The Evolution of Homo Discens: Natural Selection and Human Learning. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 48, 117–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., & Piiroinen, T. (2018b). Pragmatist Methodological Relationalism in Sociological Understanding of Evolving Human Culture. In F. Dépelteau (Ed.), The Palgrave Handbook of Relational Sociology (pp. 119–141). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., & Piiroinen, T. (2018c). Relationalism. In G. Ritzer & C. Rojek (Eds.), The Blackwell Encyclopedia of Sociology. Wiley. First published online April 27, 2018. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781405165518.wbeosr090.pub2.

  • Kivinen, O., Piiroinen, T., & Saikkonen, L. (2016). Two Viewpoints on the Challenges of ICT in Education: Knowledge-Building Theory vs. a Pragmatist Conception of Learning in Social Action. Oxford Review of Education, 42, 377–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kivinen, O., & Ristelä, P. (2002). Even Higher Learning Takes Place by Doing: From Postmodern Critique to Pragmatic Action. Studies in Higher Education, 27, 419–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K., & O’Brien, M. (2010). Niche Construction Theory and Archaeology. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 17, 303–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., & Feldman, M. W. (1999). Evolutionary Consequences of Niche Construction and Their Implications for Ecology. PNAS, 96, 10242–10247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Feldman, M. W. (2001). Cultural Niche Construction and Human Evolution. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 14, 22–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Gilbert, S. F. (2008). EvoDevo and Niche Construction: Building Bridges. Journal of Experimental Zoology (Mol Dev Evol), 310B, 549–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K., Odling-Smee, J., & Myles, S. (2010). How Culture Shaped the Human Genome: Bringing Genetics and the Human Sciences Together. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11, 137–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K., & Sterelny, K. (2006). Perspective: Seven Reasons (Not) to Neglect Niche Construction. Evolution: International Journal of Evolution, 60, 1751–1762.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin, R. (2000). The Triple Helix: Gene, Organism, and Environment. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (1998). Eve Spoke: Human Language and Human Evolution. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meloni, M. (2016). Political Biology: Science and Social Values in Human Heredity from Eugenics to Epigenetics. London: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, G. B. (2007). Evo-Devo: Extending the Evolutionary Synthesis. Nature Reviews Genetics, 8, 943–949.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noë, A. (2004). Action in Perception. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noë, A. (2009). Out of Our Heads: Why You Are Not Your Brain, and Other Lessons from the Biology of Consciousness. New York: Hill & Wang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee, J., & Laland, K. N. (2011). Ecological Inheritance and Cultural Inheritance: What Are They and How Do They Differ? Biological Theory, 6, 220–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche Construction: The Neglected Process in Evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, S., Griffiths, P. E., & Gray, R. D. (2001). Cycles of Contingency: Developmental Systems and Evolution. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (2002). The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. London: Allen Lane.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popp, J. (2007). Evolution’s First Philosopher: John Dewey and the Continuity of Nature. New York: SUNY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (1975). The Meaning of ‘Meaning’. In K. Gunderson (Ed.), Language, Mind and Knowledge (pp. 131–193). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raff, R. A. (2000). EvoDevo: The Evolution of a New Discipline. Nature Reviews Genetics, 1, 74–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, R. J. (1987). Darwin and the Emergence of Evolutionary Theories of Mind and Behavior. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rorty, R. (1979). Philosophy and the Mirror of Nature. Princeton: University of Princeton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowlands, M. (1999). The Body in Mind: Understanding Cognitive Processes. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (2010). Making the Social World: The Structure of Human Civilization. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Solymosi, T. (2013). Against Representation: A Brief Introduction to Cultural Affordances. Human Affairs, 23, 594–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (1990). The Representational Theory of Mind: An Introduction. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2001). Niche Construction, Developmental Systems, and the Extended Replicator. In S. Oyama, P. E. Griffiths, & R. D. Gray (Eds.), Cycles of Contingency: Developmental Systems and Evolution (pp. 333–349). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a Hostile World: The Evolution of Human Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2005). Made By Each Other: Organisms and Their Environment. Biology and Philosophy, 20, 21–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2007). Social Intelligence, Human Intelligence and Niche Construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 719–730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2010). Minds: Extended or Scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9, 465–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2011). From Hominins to Humans: How Sapiens Became Behaviourally Modern. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 809–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2012). The Evolved Apprentice: How Evolution Made Humans Unique. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2016). Cumulative Cultural Evolution and the Origins of Language. Biological Theory, 11, 173–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stringer, C. (2012). Lone Survivors: How We Came to Be the Only Humans on Earth. New York: Times Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, B. H., & Depew, D. J. (Eds.). (2003). Evolution and Learning: The Baldwin Effect Reconsidered. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. A. (1995). Cartesian Psychology and Physical Minds: Individualism and the Sciences of the Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tero Piiroinen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Kivinen, O., Piiroinen, T. (2020). Updating Dewey’s Transactional Theory of Action in Connection with Evolutionary Theory. In: Morgner, C. (eds) John Dewey and the Notion of Trans-action. Palgrave Studies in Relational Sociology. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-26380-5_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-26380-5_7

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-26379-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-26380-5

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics