Skip to main content

When All Linguists Did not Go to the Workshop, None of the Germans but Some of the French Did: The Role of Alternative Constructions for Quantifier Scope

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Grammatical Approaches to Language Processing

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 48))

  • 393 Accesses

Abstract

In this paper, we will present crosslinguistic data on the interpretation of negation over quantifier scope in sentences like “All children did not go to the zoo.” Questionnaire data show that English as well as German speakers prefer a linear scope interpretation of the quantifier and the negation, where it is true for all children that they did not go to the zoo. French speakers, however, strongly prefer the inverse scope interpretation where some but not all children did not go to the zoo. The preference for linear scope is moreover stronger for German speakers than for English speakers. It diminishes with age for French and English, but not for German speakers. We will argue that language differences result from two constraints: the availability of a “close” alternative in the language and the topicality of a preverbal subject. An unambiguous alternative corresponding to inverse scope in the “all-not” construction can easily be achieved in English and German by fronting the negation as in “Not all children went to the zoo.” The corresponding construction is not available in standard French. A second questionnaire study shows that adding “Not…all” sentences in the experiment, thus increasing their availability, increases the linear scope preference in English. The particularly strong preference for linear scope in German will be argued to be linked to the stronger topicality of preverbal subjects in German main clauses.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    As we will show in this Chapter, inverse scope is not actually the preferred interpretation in this type of construction in English. We will come back to the question of why the word order demanding an inverse scope reading is used in English for (1) in the General Discussion.

  2. 2.

    Note, however, that we do not want to say that it is impossible to find an inverse scope preference in German (see for example Bader & Frazier, 2005, Exp. 5, for systematic inverse scope preferences). Interpretational preferences will depend on the details of the combination of quantifiers and the type of construction.

  3. 3.

    We want to thank Thomas Weskott for pointing us to this potential alternative explanation.

References

  • Amsili, P. (2009). Chaque âge n’a pas son Homère: Petite étude de corpus sur l’interaction entre négation et quantification universelle. Unpublished Manuscript, Université Paris Diderot.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bader, M., & Frazier, L. (2005). Interpretation of leftward-moved constituents: Processing topicalizations in German. Linguistics, 431(1), 49–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumann, P., Konieczny, L., & Hemforth, B. (2014). Conversational implicatures in anaphora resolution: Alternative constructions and referring expressions. In Psycholinguistic approaches to meaning and understanding across languages (pp. 197–212). Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Büring, Daniel. (1997). The meaning of topic and focus—The 59th street bridge accent. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. G., & Kar, J. (2011). Bias of quantifier scope interpretation is attenuated in normal aging and semantic dementia. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24, 411–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carminati, M. N. (2002). The processing of Italian subject pronouns (Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation). http://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations/AAI3039345.

  • Drummond, A. (2014). Ibex farm. (http://spellout.net/ibexfarm/).

  • Dwivedi, V. D. (2013). Interpreting quantifier scope ambiguity: Evidence of heuristic first algorithmic second processing. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e81461. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0081461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandes, E., Luegi, P., Correa Soares, E., de la Fuente, I. & Hemforth, B. (2018). Adaptation in pronoun resolution: Evidence from Brazilian and European portuguese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000569

  • Filippova, K., & Strube, M. (2007). The German vorfeld and local coherence. Journal of Logic Language and Information, 16, 465–485. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10849-007-9044-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. D. (1982). The mental representation of quantifiers. In S. Peters & E. Saarinen (Eds.), Processes, beliefs, and questions. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: D. Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J. (2003). Effect displays in R for generalised linear models. Journal of Statistical Software, 8(15), 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2011). An R companion to applied regression (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L. (1999). On sentence interpretation. Studies in theoretical psycholinguistics. Springer: Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. (1995). Construal. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gennari, S. P., & MacDonald, M. C. (2006). Acquisition of negation and quantification: Insights from adult production and comprehension. Language Acquisition, 13, 125–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilboy, E., Sapena, J., Clifton, C., & Frazier, L. (1995). Argument structure and association preferences in Spanish and English compound NPs. Cognition, 54, 131–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole and J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics: Vol. 3 Speech acts. New York: Academic Press, pp. 41–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hemforth, B., Konieczny, L., Scheepers, C., Colonna, S., Schimke, S., Baumann, P., et al. (2010). Language specific preferences in anaphor resolution: Exposure or gricean maxims? In Proceedings of the 32nd annual conference of the cognitive science society. Portland, USA, August 11–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hemforth, B., Fernandez, S., Clifton, C., Frazier, L., Konieczny, L., & Walter, M. (2015). Relative clause attachment in German, English, Spanish and French: Effects of position and length. Lingua. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2015.08.010.

  • Ioup, G. (1975). The treatment of quantifier scope in a transformational grammar. Doctoral Dissertation, The City University of New York, NY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J. (2001). The dimensions of topic-comment. Linguistics, 1, 111–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1969). On understanding logically complex sentences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21, 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katsos, N., Cummins C., Ezeizabarrena M-J., Gavarro A., Kraljević J. K., Hrzica, G., et al. (2016). Cross-linguistic patterns in the acquisition of quantifiers. PNAS 13(33), 201601341, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1601341113.

  • Kemtes, K. A., & Kemper, S. (1999). Aging and resolution of quantifier scope effects. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 54, P350–P360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurtzman, H., & MacDonald, M. (1993). Resolution of quantifier scope ambiguities. Cognition, 48(1993), 243–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marandin, J.-M., Beyssade, C., Delais-Roussarie, E., & Rialland, A. (2002). Discourse marking in French: C accents and discourse moves. In Proceedings of speech prosody.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molnár, V. (1991). Das TOPIK im deutschen und im ungarischen. Stockholm, Sweden: Almqvist and Wiksell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neukom-Hermann, A. (2016). Negation, quantification and scope. A corpus study of English and German all … not constructions. University of Zurich, Faculty of Arts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noveck, I., Le Guelminger, R., Georgieff, N., & Labruyere, N. (2007). What autism can reveal about every … not sentences. Journal of Semantics, 24, 73–90. https://doi.org/10.1093/jos/ffl009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paterson, K. B., Filik, R., & Liversedge, S. (2008). Competition during the processing of quantifier scope ambiguities: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 459–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinhardt, T. (1982). Pragmatics and linguistics: An analysis of sentence topics. Philisophica, 27, 53–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunstall, S. L. (1998). The interpretation of quantifiers: Semantics and processing. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Department of Linguistics, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urbach, T. P., DeLong, K. A., & Kutas, M. (2015). Quantifiers are incrementally interpreted in context, more than less. Journal of Memory and Language, 83, 79–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallduví, E., & Engdahl, E. (1996). The linguistic realization of information packaging. Linguistics, 34, 459–519.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Pascal Amsili, Katy Carlson, and Thomas Weskott for very helpful comments on an earlier version of this chapter. Céline Pozniak and Heather Burnett were of great help with respect to checking our materials in English and French. This work was supported by the French Research Agency (ANR-10-LABX-0083).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Barbara Hemforth .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Hemforth, B., Konieczny, L. (2019). When All Linguists Did not Go to the Workshop, None of the Germans but Some of the French Did: The Role of Alternative Constructions for Quantifier Scope. In: Carlson, K., Clifton, Jr., C., Fodor, J. (eds) Grammatical Approaches to Language Processing. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 48. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-01563-3_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-01563-3_9

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-01562-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-01563-3

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics