Skip to main content

Implicit Learning and Recursion

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Language and Recursion

Abstract

Implicit learning research has focused on learning simple structures, such as chunks, even though such structures do not capture the richness of real-world human accomplishments. In particular, music and language exhibit certain recursive features that cannot be captured by regular grammars, let alone mechanisms that learn only chunks. We show in the domains of music, language, poetry and movement that people can implicitly learn recursive grammars in ways that go beyond learning chunks or mere repetition patterns. This is supported by the fact that participants are found to generalise from training materials to novel sequences following the underlying rules. In this context we further propose a parsimony argument that states that although performance on new test items can always be explained by a catch-all finite-state or chunking mechanism, such explanations can be more complex than postulating learning a supra-finite-state mechanism in that they may postulate considerably more rules or states than necessary to explain learning. This is especially true when the finite-state rather than supra-finite-state mechanism, in order to perform on the test material, needs to acquire states or chunks not required for learning the training material. We highlight both the strength and weakness of our current evidence in this regard.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Here we use the convention in the artificial grammar learning literature in experimental psychology, with numbers referring to non-terminals and capital letters as terminals; another convention is to use capital letters for non-terminals and lower-case letters for terminals. The formal definition of a formal grammar is the 4-tuple G = (N, Σ, P, S), for which N represents the set of non-terminal symbols, Σ the set of terminal symbols (surface symbols), P the set of generative production rules and S the starting symbol (from N).

  2. 2.

    Note that the distinction between recursive and iterative algorithms involves a notion of recursion that avoids encompassing all kinds of iteration (cf. Tomalin, 2007).

  3. 3.

    Note also that the distinction between context-free and context-sensitive structures is, in practice, separated by worlds of complexity which are not adequately reflected by the simple distinction of centre-embedded and (mildly context-sensitive) cross-serial structures, e.g. A1A2A3B3B2B1 and A1A2A3B1B2B3.

References

  • Bahlmann, J., Schubotz, R. I., & Friederici, A. D. (2008). Hierarchical sequencing engages Broca’s area. NeuroImage, 42, 525–534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bigand, E., & Poulin-Charronnat, B. (2006). Are we “experienced listeners”? A review of the musical capacities that do not depend on formal musical training. Cognition, 100, 100–130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bod, R. (2002). A unified model of structural organization in language and music. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 17, 289–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R. (1978). Nonanalytic concept formation and memory for instances. Cognition and Categorization, 3, 170–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R., & Vokey, J. R. (1991). Abstract analogies and abstracted grammars: Comments on Reber (1989) and Mathews et al. (1989). Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 120(3), 316–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1956). Three models for the description of language. IRE Transactions on Information Theory, IT-2, 113–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959). On certain formal properties of grammars. Information and Control, 2(2), 137–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program (Vol. 28). Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., & Schützenberger, M. P. (1963). The algebraic theory of context free languages. In P. Braffort & D. Hirschberg (Eds.), Computer programming and formal languages (pp. 118–161). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cleeremans, A. (1993). Mechanisms of implicit learning: Connectionist models of sequence processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleeremans, A. (2006). Conscious and unconscious cognition: A graded, dynamic, perspective. In Q. Jing, M. R. Rosenzweig, G. D’Ydewalle, H. Zhang, H.-C. Chen, & K. Zhang (Eds.), Progress in psychological science around the world (Neural, Cognitive and Developmental Issues, Vol. 1, pp. 401–418). Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Vries, M., Monaghan, P., Knecht, S., & Zwitserlood, P. (2008). Syntactic structure and artificial grammar learning: The learnability of embedded hierarchical structures. Cognition, 107, 763–774.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deliège, I., Mélen, M., Stammers, D., & Cross, I. (1996). Musical schemata in real time listening to a piece of music. Music Perception, 14(2), 117–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dienes, Z., Kuhn, G., Guo, X. Y., & Jones, C. (2011). Communicating structure, affect and movement: Commentary on Bharucha, Curtis & Paroo. In P. Rebuschat, M. Rohrmeier, I. Cross, & J. Hawkins (Eds.), Language and music as cognitive systems (pp. 156–168). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dienes, Z. (2012). Conscious versus unconscious learning of structure. In P. Rebuschat & J. Williams (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp. 337–364). The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dienes, Z., & Longuet-Higgins, H. C. (2004). Can musical transformations be implicitly learned? Cognitive Science, 28, 531–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dulany, D., Carlson, R., & Dewey, G. (1984). A case of syntactical learning and judgment: How conscious and how abstract? Journal of Experimental Psychology, 113, 541–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303, 377–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T., & Friederici, A. D. (2012). Artificial grammar learning meets formal language theory: An overview. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London - Series B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1933–1955. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friederici, A. D., Bahlmann, J., Heim, S., Schubotz, R. I., & Anwander, A. (2006). The brain differentiates human and non-human grammars: Functional localization and structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(7), 2458–2463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giblin, I. (2008). Music and the generative enterprise. Situating a generative theory of tonal music in the cognitive sciences. University of New South Wales

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298, 1569–1579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, J. (2004). Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hochmann, J. R., Azadpour, M., & Mehler, J. (2008). Do humans really learn AnBn artificial grammars from exemplars? Cognitive Science, 32, 1021–1036.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstadter, D. R. (1979). Godel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid. London: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Honing, H. (2011). The illiterate listener. On music cognition, musicality and methodology. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopcroft, J. E., Motwani, R., & Ullman, J. D. (2000). Introduction to automata theory, languages and computation (2nd ed.). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (2011). What is the human language faculty?: Two views. Language, 87(3), 586–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (2009). Parallels and nonparallels between language and music. Music Perception, 26(3), 195–204. doi:10.1525/mp.2009.26.3.195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R., & Lerdahl, F. (2006). The capacity for music: What is it, and What’s special about it? Cognition, 100, 33–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jäger, G., & Rogers, J. (2012). Formal language theory: Refining the Chomsky hierarchy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 1956–1970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson, R. K., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2009). Applying an exemplar model to the artificial-grammar task: Inferring grammaticality from similarity. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 550–575.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, S. (2012). Implicit learning of Chinese tonal regularity (Unpublished dissertation). East China Normal University, China.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, S., Zhu, L., Guo, X., Ma, W., Yang, Z., & Dienes, Z. (2012). Unconscious structural knowledge of tonal symmetry: Tang poetry redefines limits of implicit learning. Consciousness & Cognition, 21, 476–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koelsch, S., Gunter, T., Friederici, A. D., & Schröger, E. (2000). Brain indices of music processing: “nonmusicians” are musical. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 520–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koelsch, S., Rohrmeier, M., Torrecuso, R., & Jentschke, S. (2013). Processing of hierarchical syntactic structure in music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. Published ahead of print September 3, 2013, doi:10.1073/pnas.1300272110.

  • Kuhn, G., & Dienes, Z. (2008). Learning non-local dependencies. Cognition, 106, 184–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, G., & Dienes, Z. (2005). Implicit learning of non-local musical rules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31, 1417–1432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lai, J., & Poletiek, F. H. (2010). The impact of adjacent-dependencies and staged-input on the learnability of center-embedded hierarchical structures. Cognition, 118, 265–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerdahl, F., & Jackendoff, R. (1983). A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Li, F., Jiang, S., Guo, X., Yang, Z., & Dienes, Z. (2013). The nature of the memory buffer in implicit learning: Learning Chinese tonal symmetries. Consciousness & Cognition, 22(3), 920–930.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lobina, D. J. (2011). “A running back” and forth: A review of recursion and human language. Biolinguistics, 5(1–2), 151–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loui, P. (2011). Statistical learning – What can music tell us? In P. Rebuschat & J. Williams (Eds.), Statistical learning and language acquisition (pp. 433–462). The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martins, M. D. (2012). Distinctive signatures of recursion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1598), 2055–2064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. S. W., Maxwell, J. P., & Eves, F. F. (2009). Marginally perceptible outcome feedback, motor learning and implicit processes. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 639–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1967). The psychology of communication. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, S. (2010). Grammatiktheorie. Tübingen: Stauffenburg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Opitz, B., & Friederici, A. D. (2003). Interactions of the hippocampal system and the prefrontal cortex in learning language-like rules. NeuroImage, 19, 1730–1737.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perfors, A., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Regier, T. (2011). The learnability of abstract syntactic principles. Cognition, 118(3), 306–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perruchet, P., & Pacteau, C. (1991). Implicit acquisition of abstract knowledge about artificial grammar: Some methodological and conceptual issues. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 120(1), 112–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perruchet, P., & Rey, A. (2005). Does the mastery of hierarchical structures distinguishes humans from non-human primates? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(2), 307–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poletiek, F. H. (2011). What in the world makes recursion so easy to learn? A statistical account of the staged input effect on learning a centre-embedded hierarchical structure in AGL. Biolinguistics, 5(1–2), 36–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pothos, E. M. (2007). Theories of artificial grammar learning. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 227–244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 118, 219–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rebuschat, P. (2008). Implicit learning of natural language syntax (PhD Dissertation). Research Centre for English and Applied Linguistics, University of Cambridge, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rebuschat, P., & Williams, J. N. (Eds.). (2011). Statistical learning and language acquisition (Vol. 1). New York: De Gruyter Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, N., McLeod, P., & Dienes, Z. (2010). Implicit knowledge and motor skill: What people who know how to catch don’t know. Consciousness & Cognition, 19, 63–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Remillard, G. (2008). Implicit learning of second-, third-, and fourth-order adjacent and nonadjacent sequential dependencies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 400–424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodriguez, P., Wiles, J., & Elman, J. (1999). A recurrent neural network that learns to count. Connection Science, 11(1), 5–40. doi:10.1080/095400999116340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M. (2007). A generative grammar approach to diatonic harmonic structure. In G. Spyridis, & A. Kouroupetroglou (Eds.). Proceedings of the 4th Sound and Music Computing Conference (pp. 97–100).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M. (2010). Implicit learning of musical structure. Experimental and computational modelling approaches (Ph.D Dissertation). Centre for Music & Science, Faculty of Music, University of Cambridge, Cambrdige, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M. (2011). Towards a generative syntax of tonal harmony. Journal of Mathematics and Music, 5(1), 35–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M., & Cross, I. (2009). Tacit tonality: Implicit learning of context-free harmonic structure. In J. Louhivuori et al. (Eds.). Proceedings of the 7th Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM 2009) (pp. 443–452).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M., & Cross, I. (2010). Narmour’s principles affect implicit learning of melody. In Demorest, et al. (eds.). Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition (ICMPC 2010).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M., & Cross, I. (2013). Artificial grammar learning of melody is constrained by melodic inconsistency. Narmour’s principles affect melodic learning. Plos ONE 8(7), e66174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M., Dienes, Z., Guo, X., & Fu, Q. (2014). Implicit learning and recursion. In F. Lowenthal & L. Lefebvre (Eds.), Language and recursion. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M., Fu, Q., & Dienes, Z. (2012). Implicit learning of recursive, hierarchical grammatical structures. PLoS ONE, 7(10), e45885. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0045885.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M., & Rebuschat, P. (2012). Implicit learning and acquisition of music. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 525–53. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rohrmeier, M., Rebuschat, P., & Cross, I. (2011). Incidental and online learning of melodic structure. Consciousness & Cognition, 20(2), 214–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R. (2001). The use of predictive dependencies in language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 493–515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R. (2002). Constraints on statistical language learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 172–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, R. B., & Dienes, Z. (2008). The conscious, the unconscious, and familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 34, 1264–1288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Servan-Schreiber, D., & Anderson, J. R. (1990). Learning artificial grammars with competitive chunking. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 592–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shanks, D. R. (2005). Implicit learning. In K. Lamberts & R. Goldstone (Eds.), Handbook of cognition (pp. 202–220). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steedman, M. (2002). Plans, affordances, and combinatory grammar. Linguistics and Philosophy, 25, 723–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steedman, M. (1996). The Blues and the abstract truth: Music and mental models. In J. Oakhill & A. Garnham (Eds.), Mental models in cognitive science: Essays in honour of Phil Johnson-Laird (pp. 305–318). Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steedman, M. J. (1984). A generative grammar for Jazz Chord sequences. Music Perception, 2(1), 52–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomalin, M. (2007). Reconsidering recursion in syntactic theory. Lingua, 117, 1784–1800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uddén, J., Ingvar, M., Hagoort, P., & Petersson, K. M. (2012). Implicit acquisition of grammars with crossed and nested non-adjacent dependencies: Investigating the push-down stack model. Cognitive Science, 36, 1078–101. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2012.01235.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vokey, J. R., & Brooks, L. R. (1994). Fragmentary knowledge and the processing-specific control of structural sensitivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 1504–1510.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Westphal-Fitch, G., Huber, L., Gómez, J. C., & Fitch, W. T. (2012). Production and perception rules underlying visual patterns: Effects of symmetry and hierarchy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 2007–2022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. N. (2009). Implicit learning. In W. C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), The new handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 319–353). Bingley, UK: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Z. Dienes .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Rohrmeier, M., Dienes, Z., Guo, X., Fu, Q. (2014). Implicit Learning and Recursion. In: Lowenthal, F., Lefebvre, L. (eds) Language and Recursion. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-9414-0_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics