Skip to main content
Log in

Postactional goal pursuit: Consequences of task completion for thought content, affect, and behavioral intentions

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Postactional goal pursuit remains an underexplored area of the mindset theory of action phases. According to the theory, individuals enter a postactional phase following the completion of an intention. This phase focuses on evaluating goal outcomes, answering questions such as: Was the goal completed successfully? Was it worth the effort? Contrasting postactional phase with predecisional and actional goal pursuit, the present research examines differences in cognitive orientation, affective responses to feedback, and functional benefits for adapting subsequent goal pursuit. Five experiments show that postactional goal pursuit engenders outcome-focused deliberation, more intense affect in reaction to the current state of goal pursuit, and increased readiness to adapt subsequent goal pursuit to experienced outcomes. Moving beyond conceptual arguments, the present work provides initial evidence that postactional goal pursuit provokes a mindset-like shift suitable for a phase-specific task of outcome evaluation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For all studies, we report all dependent measures, conditions, and data exclusions. Data collection was completed prior to analysis unless otherwise noted. Sample sizes were generally based on observed effect sizes in prior studies and convenience. When we did not conduct formal a priori power analyses, we instead present sensitivity analyses for the study. All associated data are posted online: [https://doi.org/10.17632/m7cths4v4p.1].

  2. There were no effects on emotion content of the thoughts: Phase, F(1,164) = 0.56, p = .45, d = .12, Task experience, F(1,164) = 1.80, p = .18, d = .21, and Phase × Task experience interaction, F(1,164) = 0.64, p = .43, d = .12. Emotion content in the actional phase with success experience was quite high, suggesting anticipating a positive final outcome. Thoughts about the importance of the task were higher in the failure experience than in the success experience condition, F(1,164) = 6.94, p = .009, d = .41. There was no main effect of phase F(1,164) = 1.62, p = .20, d = .20, nor any interaction, F(1,164) = 2.54, p = .11, d = .25. No main effect of either phase F(1,164) = 1.22, p = .27, d = .17, or task experience F(1,164) = 0.11, p = .74, d = .05, was observed on feasibility. The Phase × Task experience interaction was significant, F(1,164) = 4.61, p = .033, d = .34. In the actional phase, thoughts concerning feasibility tended to be more frequent following a failure experience than a success experience, t(78) = 1.75, p = .083, d = .40. In the postactional phase, there was no effect of task experience, t(86) = 1.26, p = .21, d = .27.

References

  • Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Motivation und Volition im Handlungsverlauf. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation und Handeln (pp. 309–335). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Achtziger, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2018). Motivation in the course of action. In J. Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (3rd ed.). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (2003). The effects of mindset on behavior: Self-regulation in deliberative and implemental frames of mind. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(1), 86–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 50, 248–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayer, U. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). Mindset effects on information search in self-evaluation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 313–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beckmann, J. (1994). Ruminative thought and the deactivation of an intention. Motivation & Emotion, 18, 317–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beckmann, J., & Heckhausen, H. (1988). Handlungsbewertung und Aufmerksamskeitsumschaltung Jahrbuch 1988 der Max-Plank-Gesselschaft. Munich: Vandenhoeck & Rupprecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behavior. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (pp. 65–84). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, V., Giesinger, L., Job, V., & Frank, E. (2015). The role of deliberative versus implemental mindsets in time prediction and task accomplishment. Social Psychology, 46, 104–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandstätter, V., & Schüler, J. (2013). Action crisis and cost-benefit thinking: A cognitive analysis of a goal-disengagement phase. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 543–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Büttner, O. B., Florack, A., & Göritz, A. S. (2013). Shopping orientation and mindsets: How motivation influences consumer information processing during shopping. Psychology & Marketing, 30(9), 779–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Büttner, O. B., Wieber, F., Schulz, A. M., Bayer, U. C., Florack, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2014). Visual attention and goal pursuit: Deliberative and implemental mindsets affect breadth of attention. Personality and Social Psychological Bulletin, 40, 1248–1259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S. (2003). Pleasure as a sign you can attend to something else: Placing positive feelings within a general model of affect. Cognition & Emotion, 17, 241–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97(1), 19–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheema, A., & Patrick, V. M. (2008). Anytime versus only: Mind-sets moderate the effect of expansive versus restrictive frames on promotion evaluation. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(4), 462–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dai, H., Milkman, K. L., & Riis, J. (2014). The fresh start effect: Temporal landmarks motivate aspirational behavior. Management Science, 60(10), 2563–2582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doerflinger, J. T., Martiny-Huenger, T., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2017). Planning to deliberate thoroughly: If-then planned deliberation increases the adjustment of decisions to newly available information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of counterfactual thinking. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 12, 168–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feick, D. L., & Rhodewalt, F. (1997). The double-edged sword of self-handicapping: Discounting, augmentation, and the protection and enhancement of self-esteem. Motivation & Emotion, 21(2), 147–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forgas, J. P. (2013). Don’t worry, be sad! On the cognitive, motivational, and interpersonal benefits of negative mood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 225–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, K., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Mindsets and pre-conscious open-mindedness to incidental information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1), 48–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendolla, G. H. E. (1997). Surprise in the context of achievement: The role of outcome of outcome valence and importance. Motivation and Emotion, 21, 165–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), The handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol 2, pp. 53–92). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). Mindset theory of action phases. In P. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol 1, pp. 526–545). London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Keller, L. (2016). Mindset theory. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets on illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(4), 531–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, C., Schmeichel, B. J., Mennitt, E., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2011). The expression of determination: Similarities between anger and approach-related positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 172–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1987). Wünschen — Wählen — Wollen. In H. Heckhausen, P. Gollwitzer & F. Weinert (Eds.), Jenseits des Rubikon (pp. 3–9). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind. Motivation & Emotion, 11, 101–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, M. D., de Liver, Y., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). The effects of an implemental mind-set on attitude strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(3), 396–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2017). Mindset affect risk perception and risk-taking behavior: Illusory optimism and the BART. Social Psychology, 48, 135–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kray, L. J., & Galinsky, A. D. (2003). The debiasing effect of counterfactual mind-sets: Increasing the search for disconfirmatory information in group decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91(1), 69–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakens, D. (2014). Performing high-powered studies efficiently with sequential analyses. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 701–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L. A., & Sears, P. S. (1944). Level of aspiration. In J. M. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (pp. 333–378). New York: Ronald Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maglio, S. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2014). Emotion and control in the planning of goals. Motivation and Emotion, 38(5), 620–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K. D., & McMullen, M. N. (2003). A reflection and evaluation model of comparative thinking. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 7, 244–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFarland, C., & Ross, M. (1982). Impact of causal attributions on affective reactions to success and failure. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 43(5), 937–946.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFarlin, D. B., & Blascovitch, J. (1984). On the Remote Associates Test (RAT) as an alternative to illusory performance feedback: A methodological note. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 5, 223–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nenkov, G. Y., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2012). Pre-versus postdecisional deliberation and goal commitment: The positive effects of defensiveness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 106–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nummenmaa, L., & Niemi, P. (2004). Inducing affective states with success-failure manipulations: A meta-analysis. Emotion, 4(2), 207–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puca, R. (2004). Action phases and goal setting: Being optimistic after decision making without getting into trouble. Motivation and Emotion, 28(2), 121–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puca, R. M. (2001). Preferred difficulty and subjective probability in different action phases. Motivation and Emotion, 25(4), 307–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rahn, J., Jaudas, A., & Achtziger, A. (2016a). To plan or not to plan: Mindset effects on visual attention in decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 9, 109–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rahn, J., Jaudas, A., & Achtziger, A. (2016b). A mind for money: Dynamic mindset effects on smart risk taking. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 9, 145–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, C. (2012). Self-protection. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (2nd ed., pp. 327–353). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine own self be good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self be true, and to thy own self be better. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol 29, pp. 209–269). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1995). Effects of mindset on positive illusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 213–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y. (1986). Self-enhancement and self-assessment in achievement behavior. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 350–378). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440–463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y., & Pomerantz, E. M. (1998). Resolving conflicts among self-evaluative motives: Positive experiences as a resource for overcoming defensiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 22(1), 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 57, 660–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vann, R. J., Rosa, J. A., & McCrea, S. M. (2018). When consumers struggle: Action crisis and its effects on problematic goal pursuit. Psychology and Marketing. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(4), 548–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyer, R. S., & Xu, A. J. (2010). The role of behavioral mind-sets in goal-directed activity: Conceptual underpinnings and empirical evidence. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 20(2), 107–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, A. J., & Wyer, R. S. (2008). The comparative mind-set from animal comparisons to increased purchase intentions. Psychological Science, 19(9), 859–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xu, A. J., & Wyer, R. S. (2012). The role of bolstering and counterarguing mind-sets in persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(5), 920–932.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sean M. McCrea.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

McCrea, S.M., Vann, R.J. Postactional goal pursuit: Consequences of task completion for thought content, affect, and behavioral intentions. Motiv Emot 42, 852–870 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9713-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9713-3

Keywords

Navigation